Waves,
Pulses, and the Theory of Neural Masses
Walter
J. Freeman
I. Introduction II. Comparison of the Hypotheses of Reflex
Centers and of Pulse Logic A. The Origins of Reflex Centers B. The Rise of Pulse Logic C. To the Future: Divergence and Convergence D. Analog-to-Digital and Digital-to-Analog Conversions E. Mutual Inhibition and Mutual Excitation F. Summary III. State Variables of Neural Masses A. The Hierarchy of Complexity B. Topology of Olfactory Bulb Connections C. The Topology of Cortical Connections D. Neural Activity Density E. Experimental Input-Output Variables F. The Activity Density Function (a.d.f.) G. Summary IV. The Parameters of Neural Masses A. Forward Gain B. Feedback Gain C. Monotonic Response Patterns of Populations D. Oscillatory Responses of Populations within Cartels E. Feedback in the Twilight Zone F. The Distances of Massive Interactions G. Tractile Divergence H. Synaptic Divergence I. Summary II. Some Implications for Neural
Information Processing A. What Is the Wave-Pulse Duality? B. How Might Neural Vector Fields Be Generated and
Received? C. Do Traveling Waves Exist in Neural Masses? D. Are Neural Mass Actions First-Order, Second-Order, or
Epiphenomenal Events? E. Propositions for a Theory of Neural Masses VI. Summary Notes References I. Introduction It is a truism that systems
as complex as vertebrate nervous systems are more than the sum of their parts.
What is meant is that the interconnection of numbers of neurons gives rise to
collective properties belonging to the neural populations and not to the
neurons taken one at a time. The purpose of this essay is to explore some
facets of the nature of neural collective properties. Conventional wisdom holds
that such properties emerge from the interconnection of finite numbers of
neurons in discrete chains and networks, which are logical and anatomical
counterparts of the Jacksonian-Sherringtonian heirarchy of reflex arcs.
According to a popular analogy, neurons are like the electronic components of a
television receiver which can be connected in a certain way or set of ways to
give the properties of the receiver. The central thesis of this essay is the idea
that, when neurons strongly interact in sufficiently large numbers (on the
order of 10' or more), new collective properties emerge that demand a different
kind or level of conceptualization. An analogy equivalent to
that given above is the notion that temperature and pressure exist only for a
mass, in contrast to the thermal kinetic energy of molecules in the mass. The
suggestion is that certain interactive phenomena in vertebrate brains occur
only as broadly distributed and continuous events or waves across masses of
neurons, and that in some instances these cooperative phenomena may be
essential aspects of normal brain function. The task is to describe some of
these wave phenomena in terms of underlying collective properties, and to do so
in such a way as to minimize confusion between observables and principles.
Again by analogy, brain potentials (EEG waves) appear to have somewhat the
relation to wave activity of neural masses that flow patterns have to
temperature and pressure waves in atmospheric storms. They are observable side
effects that are of interest mainly because they give access to the internal
dynamics. The approach used is to
review the historical interplay between ideas concerning neural networks and
masses, to develop a set of rules for describing neural masses as dynamic
entities, and then to discuss some of the implications of those rules for
neurophysiology. Throughout the development
the emphasis is placed on the idea of graded neural synaptic interaction,
because it is interaction of neurons that gives rise to something more than the
sum of parts. Neurons are connected to each other by structural synaptic
linkages. For each neuron there is a certain density of these anatomical
connections, referring to the number and size of contacts of each neuron with
its neighbors within each unit volume of neural mass. But the significant
quantity is the momentary functional or effective connection density, which
denotes the level of transfer of influence across a given set of connections at
a given time and place. If, for example, a volley arrives on an afferent path
to a neuron that is in an absolute refractory state, the functional connection
density is zero, even though the anatomical connection density is nonzero. Two kinds of massive
connections are distinguished. The first is a one-way or forward connection
from one neuron to neurons in another mass; the second is feedback connection
of one neuron with many others in the same mass. Both types give rise to mass
actions of many neurons, but only the second gives rise to the collective
properties of interest in the present context. That is, neural interactions
based on functional interconnection densities give rise to wave phenomena, and,
as is shown for some of the neural masses in the mammalian olfactory system,
the observable effects of wave patterns in turn provide the means for measuring
the intensities of interactions. II. Comparison of the Hypotheses of Reflex
Centers and of Pulse Logic A. THE ORIGINS OF REFLEX CENTERS The basic assumption
required for a theory of neural masses is that there can exist across an
interactive mass of neurons a continually changing state of activity, which is
manifested in several forms of unit and field potentials, and in muscle
contraction and hormone secretion, but which is not identical with any of them.
The precursors of this assumption can be traced (Brazier, 1959, p. 33) to an
idea first proposed in 1784 by George Prochaska, a Bohemian
philosopher-physician, who used the term vis nervosae in direct analogy to
Isaac Newton's vis gravitans (see note 1*). He used it to connote an elemental
form of energy, which was unobservable except through its effects (such as
"reflexions" or reflexes), and conformed to natural laws that could
be quantitatively described (as could the energies of optics and gravitation),
but which could not be "explained." By mid-nineteenth century the
terms "nerve energy," "nerve force, " and reflex were in
common use. Spencer, for example, in 1863 stated (quoted from Darwin, 1872, p.
71) as an "unquestionable truth that, at any moment, the existing quantity
of liberated nerve-force, which in an inscrutable way produces in us that state
we call feeling must expend itself in some direction . . . must generate an
equivalent manifestation of force somewhere." Darwin (1872) remarked:
"This involuntary transmission of nerve force may or may not be
accompanied by consciousness. Why the irritation of nerve-cells should generate
or liberate nerve force is not known; but that this is the case seems to be the
conclusion arrived at by all the greatest physiologists such as Muller,
Virchow, Bernard, etc." (p. 70). Two developments terminated
the general acceptance of these terms. One was the popular confusion of
"nerve force" with "vital force," such that neither
survived the revulsion against vitalism by the beginning of this century. The
confusion was inevitable, because with better understanding of thermodynamics
the term "nerve energy" became oxymoronic; for example, whereas the
law of conservation of energy developed by Helmholtz in and after 1847 became
fundamental in physics, "conservation of nerve energy" remained
meaningless. The other was the
widespread acceptance, largely owing to the work of Ramón y Cajal (1911), of
the neuron doctrine as the central dogma of neurophysiology. The concept of the neuron
as a discrete structural and functional entity had earlier been proposed by
Deiters in 1865 and by Waldeyer in 1891 (who coined the word neuron), on the
basis of microscopic dissection and staining of cells in the nervous system.
This hypothesis was, in fact, an extension to the nervous system of the cell
doctrine of Schwann and Virchow developed in the 1830's, which asserted the
cellular basis for the organization of all living tissue. The concept did not
seem immediately valid for the nervous system, because neurons had
"bizarre" radiating filamentous shapes, unlike the cylinders, discs,
and spheres of almost all other cell types (Peters et al., 1970). The most
compelling kinds of evidence for its validity through the first four decades of
this century were the patterns of degeneration and regeneration of neurons
following experimental injury to central and peripheral tracts and nerves
(Ramón Cajal, 1928) ; the physiological differences distinguishing
transmission inside neurons along fibers and between neurons across synapses
(Sherrington, 1906) ; and the electrical properties of the nerve membrane
(Katz, 1939). So strong was this accumulated indirect evidence that
confirmation under the electron microscope of the anatomical integrity of the
nerve cell membrane at the synapse (Palay, 1956) was received with little
fanfare. * Notes to the text appear at the end of the
chapter on pp. 152-161. Throughout the last six
decades most neurophysiologists have inferred that, because the neuron was the
unit of growth, metabolism, and structure in the nervous system, it was also
the unit of function. When "function" meant the action of generating
and transmitting synaptic current and the nerve impulse, this inference was
quite satisfactory. But when the term "function" contained reference
to the mechanism of reflex operations (Sherrington, 1925, 1929) and animal
behavior (Hebb, 1949) or even to the physiological operations of multineuronal
parts of the nervous system (Lorente de Nó, 1938), there were difficulties.
They stemmed from the anatomical facts that the numbers of neurons in
vertebrate central nervous systems were known to be astronomically large, on the
order of 107 to 1011 and that the numbers of synapses on the surface of
each neuron, ranging from 101 to 104 or more, set the levels of possible
interactions between neurons beyond the range of numerical evaluation or
sequential observation. The question has been, Can
patterns of animal or reflex behavior be described or accounted for in terms of
finite and discrete networks of single neurons as in reflex arcs? Or is it
necessary to work with some intervening level of neural organization consisting
of interconnected neurons forming a mass? This general question can be more
sharply put in neurophysiological terms of connectivity. Granted that there are
large numbers of neurons and anatomical connections in the neural masses of the
brain, these facts do not state how many are obligatorily active during a
behavioral event. The proper question is, What is the level of functional
connection density among the neurons in a mass? If it is generally low but with
high transmission effectiveness across a selected few synapses at any moment,
then the network description is appropriate, because the neural mass exists
anatomically but not functionally. If it is generally high, then the network is
an illusion, and the appropriate level of analysis of neural "function"
must be, in some sense, the neural mass and not the single neuron. The need for some kind of
organizing concept or principle between lie level of the neuron and that of the
whole brain was met empirically for or many years by the idea of the
"reflex center." The "center" originated in the 1870's
(Brazier, 1959; Freeman, 1961) as the notation for the structure or anatomical
location in the brain, at which electrical stimulation yielded a reproducible
motor response. Under the influence of the doctrine of specific nerve energies
then prevailing, the concept was broadened to the extent that the behaviorally
related functions of the brain were commonly described in terms of a mosaic of
"centers," which were cortical or nuclear masses of neurons thought
to control some motor or sensory pattern of behavior, whether autonomic
(respiration, body temperature, etc.) or psychic (speech, vision, rage, etc.).
Each center was conceived to be the focus of nerve activity or energy, which on
release in the form of action potentials in response to an appropriate sensory
trigger would excite motor neurons to generate a specific pattern of behavior.
Connections were conceived on the model of the telephone switchboard.
Experimental verification was by use of the implanted macroelectrode. The
effect of electrical stimulation was artificially to excite neurons in the
center and cause them to produce the specific behavior. The electrode was also
used to record the electrical activity generated in the center during normally
induced patterns of behavior, and, by use of very strong electrical current, to
destroy the center and prevent its energy release and the appearance of its
controlled output. Sherrington (1906, 1925,
1929; Denny-Brown, 1940) devised an explicit model for the dynamics of the
"center" by treating it as a pool of neurons receiving converging
sets of afferent nerve fibers and transmitting pulses over a compound efferent
nerve tract. The state of activity of the pool was described as the sum of the
central excitatory and central inhibitory states (c.e.s. and c.i.s.) induced by
afferent volleys. These hypothetical energy states were graded in proportion to
input intensities between the limits of threshold and occlusion imposed by
refractory periods. They were added "algebraically" by processes of
convolution in both time and space, termed temporal and spatial summation. (In
the narrowest of several senses, Sherrington's famous title in 1906, "The
Integrative Action of the Nervous System," implied literally the summation
of infinitesimals.) The resultant determined the magnitude of pulse output from
the pool, which was estimated by measuring the magnitude of muscle contraction.
In subsequent work by others (see Note 2) the c.e.s. was identified either with
pools of action potentials (Lorente de Nó, 1938) or with pools of synaptic
potentials (Eccles, 1957, 1964). This model was widely
disseminated in textbooks of the period, and its main use was to spur the study
of the mechanisms in synaptic transmission and of the electrotonic properties
of neurons underlying processes of spatial and temporal convergence and
summation. Despite the concomitant discovery and reportage of the
electroencephalogram (EEG) by Berger in 1929, Sherrington's model did not lead
to the development of understanding of wave phenomena in the nervous system.
The reason appears in retrospect to have been that his model treated neural
events within masses only in terms of forward actions of one set of neurons on
another, and it did not allow for local reactions or interactions within pools,
but only between pools, as the basis for recurrent or cyclical neural events.
It was a statistical mechanical model of neural mass action, in which each
neuron in a pool was assumed to be identical to all others and topologically to
lie in parallel with them. Graded response properties emerged from the
all-or-none characteristics of the parts by virtue of distributions of
thresholds and refractory periods. The actions of immense numbers of neurons
could be sampled, measured, and modeled by means of lumped circuit or block
flow diagrams. The required conceptual bridge was thereby established between
the single neuron as the element of neural "function" and highly
organized reflex response patterns involving very many neurons as the element
of behavioral "function." But wave actions were not
intrinsic to the model and had to be initiated by synchronized afferent
volleys. Repetitive response patterns such as clonic afterdischarges, pendular
reflexes, the scratch reflex, and respiration could be dealt with only in terms
of bulk interactions of neuronal pools forming reciprocal centers
(flexion-extension, inspirationexpiration, heat-cold, etc.). In brief,
Sherrington's model made it possible to "explain" global neural response
patterns in terms of the properties of single neurons, but his neural masses
were internally noninteractive. Reciprocal antagonisms and facilitations were
between masses and not within masses. His model could account for the
continuous gradations and algebraic additivity (what is now called linearity or
superimposibility) of the neural input-output relations of masses of highly
nonlinear elements (the neurons), but only if continuity were imposed by the
input, owing to the absence of interactive terms. This accounts for the supreme
importance in the doctrine of centers of the use of the inductorium and the
focal stimulating electrode, which was the commonest means for imposing
synchronous volleys on numerous spatially contiguous axons. B. THE RISE OF PULSE LOGIC After 1940 the widespread
use of the microelectrode and its appurtenant technology (electronic
amplifiers, oscilloscopes, etc.) had a revolutionary impact on
neurophysiological theories of behavior. The nerve impulse was observed in almost
all parts of the peripheral and central nervous systems and came to be regarded
as the universal currency, whether it was induced by electrical stimulation or
afferent sensory stimulation or occurred "spontaneously" (see Note
3). For some microphysiologists
the microelectrode was merely the means for observing the unit pulse correlates
of neurons in centers in relation to the appropriate forms of behavior, as, for
example, the bursts of firing of neurons in the medullary reticular formation
in relation to respiration. For others a much more exciting range of analysis
was opened to view by the work of McCulloch and Pitts (1943), which showed that
pulse generators connected in networks could in theory perform certain logical
operations and gave rules for constructing such networks. They and others
(Householder and Landahl, 1945) rapidly extended this concept to networks
containing erratically firing units without precise time relationships among
them. In 1949 Hebb, with the aid of neuroanatomical concepts of Lorente de Nó
(1938), used these operations for the analysis of human and animal. behavior
(see Note 4). Owing largely to his work, and with the concurrence of
neuroanatomists and neuroembryologists, who like Sperry (1951) strongly
emphasized the meticulous detail with which neural connections are laid down,
the hypothesis of the nervous system as a pulse-logic device superseded the
older concept of the brain as a switchboard of centers (see Note 5). The payoff from this change
in theory was rapid and continuing, especially in the visual system (H. B.
Barlow, 1953; Jung, 1961; Hubel and Wiesel, 1963), and to lesser extents in the
somesthetic (Mounteastle, 1957) and auditory (Kiang, 1968) systems of
vertebrates, the eye of Limulus (Hartline, 1938; Hartline and Ratliff, 1958),
and a variety of invertebrate preparations (Bullock and Horridge, 1965). It is
now well established that transmission through each stage in a sensory channel
is not simply by forward relay but involves some logical operations, which can
be conceived as the addition and subtraction of pulses on adjacent lines
(axons), selective delay, clipping by thresholds, etc., performed by
interneurons and recurrent collateral axons. In some channels these processes
have been shown to have precise spatial localization and very fine-grain
texture. Among the most compelling neurophysiological demonstrations of the
past decade have been the remarkable specificities of the input configurations
that serve optimally to drive selected neurons in the visual and somesthetic
pathways. These patterns have been
termed "trigger features" (H. B. Barlow, 1953) ; the operation of the
neural nets has been termed "feature" abstraction (Lettvin et al.,
1959). Logical calculi have been devised (e.g., Rosenblatt, 1962) to simulate
the behaviorally related functions of such nets in terms of the selected
storage, recognition, and recall of "features." A series of
statistical procedures has been developed to obtain useful in formation from
neural pulse trains (Gerstein and Kiang, 1960; Rodieck et al., 1962; Perkel and
Bullock, 1968). H. B. Barlow (1969) and Marr (1969) have taken the theory to
its logical culmination, as McCulloch and Pitts did earlier (1943), in
proposing that perception and motor action are based on serially ordered neural
nets, such that single neurons at successive removes from primary receptors and
effectors have progressively more complex input or output connections and
trigger features or motor correlates, until, at the highest level, a single
neuron can "encode" a unique percept, memory, or action, which
McCulloch and Pitts (1943) termed the "psychon." Current experimental
work is directed toward defining the trigger features of higher-order cells,
analyzing the simpler networks of lower-order neurons to determine how the
operations are performed, and determining how the networks are embryologically
laid down with the requisite degree of anatomical specificity. C. TO THE FUTURE: DIVERGENCE AND
CONVERGENCE It is proposed that the
most fruitful development will next occur as a fusion of the concepts of reflex
centers and pulse logic, which are alike in several important ways. Each in its
time has been an attempt to conceive and understand brain function in terms of
brain properties. Neither is an analogy in the way of the telephone
switchboard, the Faraday field, the servo system, the digital computer, the
correlograph, or the holograph. Each has been very fruitful as the stimulus and
guide to experimental work, because it has been so directly tied to a technique
of observation on the brain. Each has been developed around the idea that an
on-going pattern of animal behavior can be identified with an on-going pattern
of cerebral activity, which is observable through patterns of neuroelectrical
activity. But each has suffered the limitations of its technique. The
description of the operation of centers was never extended past the global
summation of Sherringtonian central excitatory and inhibitory states, largely
because the macroelectrode did not give access to the rich texture of neural
events within "reflex centers." With the microelectrode it is very
difficult to make effective observations on more than one neuron at a time, and
still more difficult to make or test effective inferences concerning the operation
of large numbers of neurons surrounding the one under observation. Herein lies an important
limitation of pulse-logic models of the nervous system. The microelectrode as
conventionally used is a tool for the study of convergence in the nervous system,
whereas the operations of the system are based on divergence. As such, the tool
gives a deceptively deterministic view of neural events. Each neuron in the
brain at any moment must have some set of effective input connections
(including the null set) from other parts of the brain, including sensory
receptors. It must have some instantaneous pulse rate (including zero).
Corresponding to the set of input connections there must be some configuration
of afferent stimuli, which serves maximally to enhance or decrease the pulse
rate. That is its trigger feature. On the other hand, of all possible motor
actions there must be one that is associated with maximal firing rate. That is
its motor correlate or psychon under the designated conditions of observation. Each stimulus is invariably
a surface event over an array of receptors. Even where elaborate precautions
are taken to restrict input to a single receptor, the input configuration must
be specified as "this neuron on" and all others "off" or
"at background level." Each afferent axon characteristically branches
'repeatedly, and if its own spatial divergence is minimal the divergence at
synaptic stages is prominent. Each motor response consists in timed sequences
of muscle contractions and relaxations in response to space-time patterns of
motor neuron firing. These patterns can also be conceived as surface events in
the array of neurons sustaining axons to the periphery. No one denies that even
the simplest stimulus-response event consists in surface-to-surface
transmission of pulses from arrays of receptors through intervening synaptic
layers to arrays of motor neurons. However, the numbers of
neurons in neural arrays characteristically are extremely large, and the degree
of overlap of the fibers of each neuron with its neighbors is very high.
Microelectrodes cannot be used to sample more than a very few neurons among the
thousands or millions of neurons constituting a typical mass. Pulse-logic
concepts do not in themselves yield rules for spatial and temporal sampling
through a neural mass. It is true that experimenters devise probability
distributions, topographic maps, homunculi, and other continuous patterns to
describe the most probable locations of neurons in arrays triggered by or
associated with certain classes of stimulus or response, but these are based on
past experience and on anatomical analysis, not on the theory. It is proposed that these
"arrays" of neurons might not merely be describable in terms of
continuous distributions, but in many cases might actually function that way.
Concepts of mass action might take the form of probability distributions of
pulses, which would allow description of the activity of single neurons in the
context of the mass. D. ANALOG -TO-DIGITAL AND DIGITAL-TO-ANALOG
CONVERSIONS It is well-known that
neurons are not pulse-logic devices (Bullock, 1959). They transmit over long
distances (that is, distances greater than a few tens or hundred of microns) by
means of pulses, but the significant interactions within neurons at synaptic
junctions in masses are not impulsive. They occur in the form of continuously
varying synaptic currents, which determine from moment to moment whether or not
a neuron will fire a pulse. Each neuron is a two-stage converter. It receives
pulses and converts them at synapses into synaptic currents. It filters these
currents through the distributed resistance and capacitance of its dendritic
tree and adds the weighted sum. It converts this to a pulse train, or, more
accurately, to a probability of pulse formation at some site or multiple sites
of optimal sensitivity, termed trigger zones. The pulse is transmitted to other
neurons in (usually) all-or-none fashion, but with logical processing in the
form of translation, delay, dispersion, and "multiplication" over
successively branching axon terminals (see Note 6). The identity of manifold
input pulses on dendritic trees is lost in terms of their times and locations.
Discrete values are smoothed by the cable properties of dendrites, and the output
of any one neuron can. only represent the weighted, smoothed sum of afferent
pulses over its entire surface and for some uncertain period of time. That is,
the control of the pulse rate is invested in an analog signal, which varies
continuously and not discretely in its temporal and spatial dimensions. The combination of
divergence in neural transmission with two-stage conversion by each neuron
brings an uncertainty. A sensory stimulus evoked neural discharge runs out over
diverging axons leaving one neuronal array and approaching another array. The
neurons of the next array are activated by the volley, but their output pulse
trains contain little information about which axons in the now converging array
were necessary and sufficient to produce the output. If the involved axons are
described as a set, and the neurons of which the axons are a part are
collectively described as a neural mass, there are (for present purposes) two
modes of transmission by the mass with differing implications for the observer. If the behaviorally related
neural transmission conforms to Hebb's (1949) view, then on each of several
parallel axons the message is complete. A record of the pulse train on any one
axon will provide the observer with all the information that was sent and
received. But if the transmission occurs partially on each of several axons in
random order, uncertain and indifferent in number, location, and timing within
not well defined limits, as envisioned by Lashley (1942), then the observer
cannot know from a record of one pulse train what message was sent. He must
either back up in time and repeat the transmission again and again, until '-,he
axon he observed has (presumably) displayed on the average the same behavior as
all the other axons during any one transmission, or he must record
simultaneously from all the active axons. Because he cannot know their
identities, locations, or number, he cannot logically achieve either of these
ends, unless he records from all the axons in the set, nonactive as well as
active. If there are more than 100 or so, he is forced to express his findings
in the form of a probability distribution around the neuron under observation,
which is a continuous function locally across both space and time. How is an observer to know
whether he is recording from a Hebb neuron or a Lashley neuron? Within the
present context of neuropsychology and electrophysiology he cannot, because he
has the unrestricted freedom to structure the experimental situation to yield
whichever answer he desires. He usually desires dramatic bursts of pulses and
strives for them. But the list of possible neural pulse codes is long and still
increasing (Perkel and Bullock, 1968), and there is no a priori reason given by
pulse logic to prefer some over others. The identification of pulse rate with
psychon intensity is appropriate for first-order sensory neurons (Fulton,
1949), but beyond that level it is merely a plausible analogy. The aim of a
higher level of analysis should be to describe the properties of cooperative
activity as they appear in neural masses in analog form, to deduce the
equations governing an alog-to-digital and digital-to-analog conversions, and
then to predict the admissible forms of pulse codes. E. MUTUAL INHIBITION AND MUTUAL EXCITATION Another limitation of the
pulse-logic hypothesis is its failure to incorporate the property of excitatory
feedback among neurons. The defining characteristic of an interactive neural
mass is the functional interconnectivity of a large enough number of neurons to
make feasible a continuum as an approximation to their function. How large that
number might be is not important; elsewhere it has been suggested that the
numbers from 101 or 101 to 101 or 101 are useful guidelines (Freeman, 1972).
The definition must also include a statement on the kind of interaction,
whether excitatory, inhibitory, or a mixture of both. An array of sensory neurons
interacting by mutual inhibition and giving rise to the phenomenon of
"lateral" or "surround" inhibition has been described for
the retina of an invertebrate, Limulus (Hartline and Ratliff, 1958; Knight et
al., 1970). This pattern of interaction leading to contrast enhancement along
lines and edges has become virtually an archetype for the interpretation of
interactions of neurons in the vertebrate retina, even though the latter
contain both excitatory and inhibitory neurons. That is, emphasis has been laid
on the inhibitory actions of neurons, such that each neuron is thought
primarily to inhibit its neighbors and, by reciprocal release from the
inhibition exerted by its neighbors, to enhance its own activity. This analysis fits well
with the conception of each neuron as a separate information channel. There is
minimal channel cross talk, confusion between adjacent channels, or loss of
topographic specificity at the readout end, and yet each neuron has the local
cross-connections demanded for local logical processing. A very different pattern of
function emerges from neurons that are interconnected by excitatory synaptic
contacts. There is strong evidence in the olfactory system (Freeman, 1968a,b)
and possibly other parts of the brain that dense interconnections occur among
excitatory neurons. That is, excitatory neurons excite each other and are
re-excited in return. This kind of interconnection has, been modeled in terms
of pulse logic as the reverberating circuit, the two-neuron or multineuron
closed loop with a periodically circulating pulse (Lorente de Nó, 1938; Hebb,
1949). In neural masses the recursion of an impulse of a neuron back to itself
through its neighbors can be modeled in terms of a one-dimensional diffusion
process (Freeman, 1964). That is, the recurrent event is dispersed by Gaussian
distributions of conduction, synaptic, and cable delays and is smoothed by the
resistive-capacitative properties of the membrane. A large number of such
interconnected neurons cannot "reverberate" (generate periodic pulse
trains) in the steady state, for the output of each is desynchronized at each
passage around the loop. If left to itself such a mass can only approach some
steady level of activity, each neuron generating a random pulse train at a mean
rate peculiar to itself. This condition is evident in the
"spontaneous" or background pulse trains of central neurons, which
characteristically are aperiodic and cannot be attributed to
"pace-makers" or to discrete loops. If a pulse is introduced
into such an excitatory mass by way of an afferent volley on a set of input
axons, to have a detectable effect on any one neuron it must be carried on
enough axons and arrive within a short enough period of time that the arrival
is distinct from a random surge of background activity delivered to that
neuron. That is, multiple afferent neurons in the local domain must be coherently
active to achieve an effect on any one neuron, and (in such a mass) the same
effect must take place in more than one neuron, if it takes place in any, owing
to afferent divergence, though to varying degrees. Recurrent excitation in the
activated region of the mass must lead to further coherent activity (dispersed
in time as well as divergent in the surface), which is an increase in
probability of aperiodic firing of neurons over the domain of interconnection.
The transmitted event to other neurons in the mass is a spatially and
temporally distributed set of pulses on the axons of those neurons that were
likely in any case to have fired. Owing to the smoothing of a spatiotemporal
distribution of pulses by the dendritic membranes of the recipient cells, the
transmitted event is a continuous variable. That is, it cannot be treated as a
pulse or sum of pulses. It is a wave. The probability of firing
of each neuron is determined by the local value of this wave, superimposed on
background activity, but the same wave occurs in many neurons in the same
domain. The sum of probabilities of firing over those neurons gives rise to an
actual pulse density (number of pulses per unit area per unit time), which is
the output. Owing to temporal dispersion and divergence of axons this event
must likewise undergo smoothing within an axonal tract, so that the output not
only can but must be treated as a duality. In terms of its accessibility to
observation with a microelectrode, it is a pulse function-that is, a set of near-random
pulse trains with some coherence in terms of mean firing rate in relation to an
afferent stimulus. In terms of its effect on the next succeeding synaptic
layer, the output is a wave function-a continuous event in space and time (see
Note 7). F. SUMMARY The earliest attempt to
represent neural dynamics in the abstract led to the concept of "nerve
energy" as the basis for neural action in relation to sensation and
motion. The term fell into disuse with the advent of the neuron doctrine, but
the concept persisted as Sherrington's central excitatory state. Reflex actions
were conceived to result from the sum of concerted actions of like neurons
wired in parallel, so that neural masses could be treated as lumped elements in
block diagrams. Subsequently the technology of the microelectrode gave access
to the pulse trains generated by single neurons, and conceptually the networks
of centers were replaced by networks of single neurons. By logical extension
the operation of neurons within masses was conceived as encoding behaviorally
significant information ("psychons") into the pulse rates of single
neurons, in much the way that the discharge magnitudes of "centers"
were previously related to specific behavioral functions. It is proposed that pulse
logic can be fused with the doctrine of centers in such a way that the chief
virtue of each can be preserved and extended. This is the identification of a
behavioral event with a pattern of neural activity, which is accessible to
electrophysiological observation. Three areas are identified for extending
pulse logic into the domain of continuously varying quantities. One is the
description of divergence of neural activity in terms of spatial pulse
probability distributions. A second is the description of the rules governing
wave and pulse conversions. The third is the description of dense excitatory
neural interactions in terms of temporal pulse probability distributions. It is suggested that the
fusion of these two modes of thinking will impose a duality of conception onto
the neural pulse train, viewed with respect to the neuron as a pulse function
and with respect to the neural mass as a wave function. III. State Variables of Neural Masses An observed pulse train of
a neuron in an internally interactive mass can be treated experimentally in
either of two ways. The conditions of the observations can be adjusted to
optimize the probability of firing in relation to an afferent stimulus, which
leads to the designation of the trigger feature or "psychon" in the
context of pulse-logic analysis. Or for a known stimulus configuration the
complex covariation can be observed between the pulse train and the activity
patterns of other neurons in the local domain. Generically speaking, the search
can be made for the structure of the higher-order wave event, of which the
pulse train is a manifestation. Because the wave event is sustained by
uncertain numbers of neurons firing unpredictably in unspecified volumes of
neural tissue, that search must undertake the description of neural masses in
terms peculiar to their own properties, arising out of but distinct from the
properties of single neurons. These properties include
the types, levels, and densities of interconnections; the gross geometries and
dimensionalities of neural masses; the state variables of neural masses, which
serve to describe their levels of activity; the observed events manifesting the
state variables in the forms of pulse trains and field potentials; and the
system parameters, both fixed and variable, consisting mainly of rate, space,
and gain coefficients. Experimental freedom of
choice conveys the right to choose preparations optimal for the end in view.
The retina (both vertebrate and invertebrate), the mammalian geniculocalcarine
and lemniscal systems, and a variety of invertebrate visceral ganglia have been
found well adapted to network analysis. On the other hand, the system best
adapted to studies of neural masses is the vertebrate olfactory system. The
high numbers of neurons, degrees of divergence, and density of
interconnectedness are exemplary, whereas the degrees of topographic
specificity and specialization of cell types are minimal. Most of what now
follows applies directly although not uniquely to the mammalian olfactory bulb
and cortex. A. THE HIERARCHY OF COMPLEXITY The assumption of the
possible existence of an active state across a neural mass means that
widespread cooperative activity, not necessarily uniform, synchronous, or
intensive, is assumed to occur among its neurons. Such cooperation can result
either from a common driving source or from internal interaction, In either
case the active state is based on a form of neural connection. Analysis of the
active state requires the specification of the approximate numbers of neurons
involved, the size and limits of their domain, the types and densities of
connection, and the level of complexity of connection. The simplest type of neural
mass is a set of neurons which have no internal interactions among them, but
which have a common source of input as the basis for cooperative activity. This
type is hereafter called an "aggregate." An example is the aggregate
of olfactory receptors. These are specialized sensory neurons having cell
bodies in the olfactory mucosa in the nose. The input end of each neuron
maintains cilia projecting into the pituitous surface of the nasal cavity. The
output end is an unbranched axon, which does not send collateral axons to other
receptors. There is no neural basis for interaction among the receptors, but
there is a possible basis for covariant activity in responses to olfactory
stimuli, which are delivered to the receptor surface in a continuous air stream. It is convenient to
subdivide aggregates into three subtypes. That containing all excitatory
neurons is called an excitatory aggregate; that containing all inhibitory
neurons is an inhibitory aggregate; that containing roughly equal numbers is a
mixed aggregate. This example illustrates a
basic rule, that the defining characteristic for a neural mass is a possible
set of cooperative active states rather than any one active state. Obviously
this definition makes it possible to define the entire brain as a neural mass,
which is reasonable, but the definition is made more useful by some restrictions,
such as the rule that they have a common input. In this manner the olfactory
receptors can be conceived as a neural mass, even though it is unlikely that
they are either all covariant with each other at any moment, or all independent
of other brain neurons. In the case of cooperation
based on interconnection, the simple form of interactive mass is that in which
all the neurons are either all excitatory or all inhibitory. This type of mass
is called a "population." Each neuron need not be connected with all
others in the mass, but it must be densely connected with many neurons in its
vicinity. By "dense"' connection is meant that each neuron is
connected to a large but unspecified fraction of the neurons within the radius
of its own processes, and that no small number of such connections dominate the
whole set. By "many" neurons is meant that it is inconvenient or
impossible to count them, but that the numbers can be estimated by an
appropriate volumetric sampling procedure, and that the active states of that
number of neurons can be reasonably approximated by some continuous average
across them. The outer limits of the
mass are set by the anatomical extent of the neurons having that kind and
density of connectivity. Additionally, because the essential characteristic of
the mass is the possibility of cooperative activity, it is reasonable to impose
the requirement that the interconnected neurons receive direct (monosynaptic)
input in common from another previously defined mass, whether interconnected (a
population) or not (an aggregate). This ensures the possibility of cooperative
activity. The outer limits of these two defining characteristics would define
two sets, and the mass would be the union of the two sets. There are two types of
interactive masses or "populations." One is composed of densely
interconnected, mutually inhibitory neurons, which have some common source of
input, and which coincide with a locus for possible states of cooperative
activity. These form an "inhibitory population." The other is
similarly composed of excitatory neurons. It will be shown that the dynamics of
excitatory and inhibitory populations differ in a characteristic way, so that
it is essential to distinguish them at the outset as being each composed of
neurons having a common sign of output: all excitatory or all inhibitory. The dynamics characteristic
of a neural mass containing both types of population densely interactive
without regard to type are far more complex than the dynamics of either simple
interactive mass. At this juncture it is desirable to introduce a series of
four levels of complexity intervening between the neuron and the whole brain,
and a new vocabulary to label them. The first level is that of
the aggregate (the noninteractive mass), which is either excitatory,
inhibitory, or mixed. The second level is that of the population (interactive
mass), for which the two types are excitatory and inhibitory. The third level
is that of the compound interactive neural mass, which can be treated (for analytic
purposes) as resulting from the dense interconnection of excitatory and
inhibitory populations. (Populations containing scattered single neurons of
unlike sign of output, such as inhibitory neurons within an excitatory
population at insufficient density to establish detectable interactions with
each other, need not be reclassified, but the disparate neurons are not members
of the population, even though embedded within it. Alternatively, if the
numbers of disparate neurons are appreciable, but not their density of
interconnection, the mass might he described, for this example, as an
excitatory population in parallel or in series with an inhibitory aggregate.) Each mass existing as some
combination of populations has properties unique to itself, depending on the
nature of the interactions, which can be "explained" in terms of the
properties of the populations, but which transcend them. In all their variety
and specificity the entities deserve a class name that has not been applied to
lesser masses of neurons. It is proposed that they be called neural
"cartels," from the same root as "chart" or
"charter." The dense connection of an excitatory with an inhibitory
population is a simple cartel. A complex cartel is formed by the dense
connection of a simple cartel with an aggregate, population, or some
combination of them in series or parallel topology. This third level of
organization, that of the neural cartel, is of greatest importance in respect
to observation. Populations and covariant aggregates naturally function within
cartels. Nerve tracts normally activate complex cartels, and they can be isolated
from certain input by cutting the tracts. Surgical ablation is most practically
performed on the whole of a cartel. Therefore, a model of neural masses must
aspire to predict and describe at this level- of complexity. Although recording yields
electrophysiological data produced by aggregates and populations, the data
cannot be understood or explained without recourse to the cartel of which they
are parts. The state or the output of a cartel must be represented by multiple
neural activity distributions, at least one for each component population and
covariant aggregate. Examples from the olfactory system are described below. The fourth level of
complexity results from interaction among complex cartels. This is the level of
"brain systems." Familiar examples are the sensory and motor systems
of the vertebrate brain. The dynamics at this level have not yet been modeled
in terms of linear analysis, as have the dynamics of the first three levels
(Freeman, 1972). To summarize, the neural
mass is defined as the set of neurons lying within a locus of domains of
cooperative activity supported by them. Depending on the type, density, and
complexity of neural interconnections, a four-level hierarchy of masses within
masses can be defined. 1. The aggregate is a mass of neurons which
have some common source of input, but which do not interact with each other.
There are three subtypes depending on the sign of output: all excitatory, all
inhibitory, or mixed. 2. The population is a mass of neurons which
have some common source of input and a common sign of output, and which are
densely interconnected. There are two kinds of populations, excitatory and
inhibitory, 3. The simple cartel is formed by the dense
interconnection of two populations, one excitatory and the other inhibitory.
The complex cartel is formed by the combination of a simple cartel with a
population or an aggregate. 4. The brain system is a modality-specific
chain or network of neural cartels. FiG. 1. This is a diagram of the neuron types
of the olfactory system and their connections. All types of connections and
feedback loops are shown. The levels of neural mass (aggregate, population,
cartel, and system) are based on these connection types. Although each
schematic neuron is shown making a few connections, each neuron in the bulb and
cortex makes the indicated types of connection innumerable times. B. TOPOLOGY OF OLFACTORY BULB CONNECTIONS The vertebrate olfactory
system can be divided into three gross parts and two connecting nerves. Each of
the parts and the nerves is accessible to stimulation, recording, and surgical
ablation. They are the mucosa, the primary olfactory nerve (PON), the olfactory
bulb, the lateral olfactory tract (LOT), and the prepyriform or primary
olfactory cortex (see Fig. 1). The neural masses within these gross parts
display clear organization into sequential layers (Ramón y Cajal, 1955;
Valverde, 1965). The first part of the
olfactory system, the layer of mucosa, contains the receptor cell bodies. Their
axons extend from the mucosa without branching and form the PON.
Electrophysiological measurements imply that the receptor neurons are all
excitatory. This, plus the fact that they are not interconnected, means that
these neurons form an excitatory aggregate. The second part is the
olfactory bulb. The bulb can be conceptualized simply as a kidney bean-shaped
body made up of several layers. The PON axons terminate in the outer layer,
which is made up of the periglomerular cells (P) and the glomeruli. The
glomeruli are spherical nests of synaptic endings. Within the glomeruli the PON
terminals connect with the mitral cells (M) and the tufted cells (T). The
periglomerular cells are excitatory and densely interconnected; thus they form
an excitatory population (P+P). The convention (Freeman,
1967) adopted for representation of a population (a densely interactive mass of
neurons which are all excitatory or all inhibitory) in a flow diagram (Fig. 2)
is a pair of circles, each with a horizontal arrow to the other. The plus sign
(+) represents excitation, and the minus sign (- ) denotes inhibition. The
first circle represents the subset of neurons in the population which receives
a given input, in this case from the PON by way of the glomeruli. This subset
will excite another subset which may include all or part of itself. One can
imagine an indefinite replication of successive excitations, but two groups
interacting among themselves will suffice for modeling purposes. The second
circle represents the subset of the neurons acted on by the first subset. The
output is represented diagrammatically as taken from the first subset, though
in precision work a correction term is required to predict the output of the
whole population. An aggregate is represented by one circle (for example, R). The next inner layers of
the bulb are made up of the mitral and tufted cells. They have two kinds of
dendrites. The apical dendrites for both cells are perpendicular to the bulbar
surface and end in thick brushes in the glomeruli, where they receive input
from the PON and the periglomerular neurons. The basal dendrites extend for
long distances in all directions parallel to the bulbar surface. The axons of
both neuron types give off collaterals ending on the cell bodies of the other
type which are excitatory (Nicoll, 1971). Thus they form an excitatory
population (M++T). The innermost layer of the
bulb is made up of granule cells (G), the most numerous type of cell. They are
slender neurons with sparsely branched dendrites oriented perpendicular to the
bulbar surface. They have no axons. They are densely studded with small bulbous
projections called spines or gemmules (Valverde, 1965), which form two-way or
reciprocal synapses with the basal dendrites of mitral and tufted cells (Rall
et al., 1966). Indirect evidence from electrophysiological recordings implies
that they are densely interconnected, and that effectively they are inhibitory
to each other (Freeman, 1972) as well as to mitral and tufted cells (Rall and
Shepherd, 1968). It is inferred that the granule cells form an inhibitory
population (G = G). The dense interconnection
of the mitral-tufted excitatory population with the granule inhibitory
population forms the bulbar cartel. The conventional form representing a simple
cartel in a flow diagram is the diagonalized square (T++MG = G). FIG. 2. This is a lumped-circuit diagram
showing the main functional connections within the two olfactory complex
cartels located in the bulb and cortex, and the polarities of connection: +,
excitation; -, inhibition; x-, multiplicative inhibition (related to
presynaptic inhibition). For symbols, see text. From Freeman (1970). The serial-parallel cascade
of the periglomerular excitatory population into the bulbar simple cartel forms
the bulbar complex cartel (P++P)(T++MG = G). Together with terminal parts of
the PON and the origins of the LOT this complex cartel constitutes the
olfactory bulb. Although the axons of the
tufted cells end predominantly if not exclusively within the bulb, the axons of
the mitral cells project from the inner layer of the bulb out to the surface of
the cortex, where they form the LOT. The LOT axons terminate in the third gross
part of the olfactory system, the cortex, so that the mitral cells are the
readout neurons of the bulb. C. THE TOPOLOGY OF CORTICAL CONNECTIONS The cortex contains three
main types of neurons (Valverde, 1965; O'Leary, 1937), which have been
categorized also in respect to their physiological properties (Freeman, 1968a).
The superficial pyramidal cells (A) form a dense sheet of cell bodies lying
parallel to the cortical surface. Their dendrites receive the axons of the LOT.
Each neuron has a multiply branched axon, which terminates on other superficial
pyramidal cells as well as on deeper cells. They are excitatory (Freeman,
1968a), so that these neurons from an excitatory population (A++A). The cortical granule cells
(B) occur in the layer deep to the superficial pyramidal cells. Their dendrites
radiate in all directions. Their axons terminate on each other as well as on
other neuron types. They are inhibitory (Freeman, 1968a) and form an inhibitory
population (B = B). The dense interconnection
of these two populations forms the cortical simple cartel (A++AB = B). The third mass is made up
of deep pyramidal cells (C). They are relatively large and sparse in number.
Their dendrites radiate in all directions and form connections mainly if not
dominantly with the cortical granule cells. Electrophysiological evidence
indicates that they are excitatory. Their axons give off collaterals to the
cortical granule cells before leaving the cortex and entering that part of the
white matter known as the external capsule (EC). They are the cortical readout
neurons. The connections between deep pyramidal neurons appear to be much less
dense than their connections to and from granule cells. Therefore the set is
provisionally classified as an aggregate (C). The serial cascade of the
cortical simple cartel into this excitatory aggregate forms another complex
cartel, (A++AB = B)(C), which (together with parts of the anterior olfactory
nucleus) constitutes the functional prepyriform cortex. There is some evidence
from EEG recording (Freeman, 1963) that the cortical mass should be subdivided
into two or more complex cartels, each receiving input from the LOT and
transmitting to the EC in parallel, but this is still uncertain. D. NEURAL ACTIVITY DENSITY The single neuron embedded
in a mass is (with few exceptions) a one-directional four-stage transmitter,
which generates activity in two forms. The dendritic tree receives pulse trains
from afferent axons at synapses and converts them to hyperpolarizing or
depolarizing dendritic current. This is transformed by weighted algebraic
summation within the dendrites to the dendritic current acting on the trigger
zone. There it is converted back to a pulse train. Finally, the axonal tree
distributes its impulses in accordance with the location of its branches.
Characteristically, the dendritic responses to two or more inputs are
superimposable; that is, they are additive and proportional to the input, so
that they are susceptible to linear analysis. Characteristically the axonal
response is all-or-none, discontinuous, and highly nonlinear. The activity of the single
neuron at any given location in time and space can be specified experimentally
by intracellular recording to determine two variables: the degree of membrane
polarization related to synaptic current, and the presence or absence of a
pulse at any time. Similarly, the activity of a neural mass can be observed
experimentally by extracellular recording to determine two variables, which are
the relative density of synaptic current (V) generated by the dendrites and the
number of pulses per unit time and volume (P) on the axons of the mass. In reality an intracellular
recording from the soma of a neuron does not specify its entire state. The
mitral cell, for example, has output through basal dendrodendritic synapses
with granule cells, axon collaterals to tufted cells, and widely distributed
axon terminals in the anterior olfactory nucleus and cortex. It is doubtful
that the level of transmembrane potential in the soma can serve fully to
specify the magnitudes of those outputs at any moment. It is not possible to
measure the transmembrane potential at many points in the neuron, so rules are
adopted (for example, an action potential is all-or-none for the entire axon;
synaptic conductance changes are nonpropagating; membrane specific capacitance
is invariant) to permit an observer to infer the state of the whole neuron from
a single measure. The validity of such rules is continually subject to
challenge and review, but they are widely used. Similarly, it is not
possible to measure neural responses at all points in a mass, and rules are
needed to extrapolate from a limited set of measurements to the active state of
the mass. But the two cases differ in regard to dimensionality. The state of a
single neuron can be inferred from one of the two variables changing with time
at some fixed point, but the active state of a mass must be inferred from one
of these variables changing in space as well as in time. The neural masses of the
olfactory system occur in layers, which lie orthogonal to the main trajectory
of axonal trunks connecting them. Interactions (when they occur) take place in
the layers, and neurons vertically separated in the same part of a layer often
have similar response patterns. One of the rules applied to masses is that the
active state can be adequately described in the two surface dimensions of the
layer containing a neural mass, so that the activity of any neuron or set of
neurons in the vertical column (Mounteastle, 1957; Hubel and Wiesel, 1963) at
or around a point on the surface of that mass serves to represent the active
state of that part of the surface. By means of this rule the neural activity at
or in the near vicinity of a point can be referred to as an activity density. The size of the "near
vicinity" in which a single value for one of the variables, V or P, can
represent the activity density of the surface is larger than the size of a single
neuronal soma and less than the radius of its dendrites. In a mass containing
an excitatory population, both P and V are continuous. This is unlike the case
for measures of activity of single cells where only one variable is
continuous-that which measures membrane potential, V. Both P and V are
continuous across the surface because the many branches of the many neurons are
interwoven and the neurons are densely interconnected. It is continuous in
time, because there is temporal dispersion across the large number of axon
terminals, and because the dendritic membranes exert a smoothing action. Owing
to this trait the activity distribution of a neural mass can be specified by
many fewer measurements than the numbers of its neurons (see Note 8). This continuity must hold
for populations but need not hold for aggregates (noninteractive masses), owing
to their lack of internal interconnectedness, unless it is imposed by
continuity in some input function, such as an electrical stimulus. Because a functional
surface is defined as the entire cross section of a population or aggregate
orthogonal to the transmission trajectory, it may coincide with the anatomical
surface of the mass. But it need not. Typical functional surfaces are indicated
by dotted lines shown in Fig. 1. The mucosa is a representative surface for the
receptor cell aggregate, but so also is any cross section through the PON. All
surfaces have limits or boundaries. The three populations of the bulb share a
common functional surface, a common anatomical surface, and a common boundary.
The boundary is determined by the distribution of the terminals of the PON.
Activity in the mitral cell population can also be represented at any cross
section through the LOT. This is an example of a population for which more than
one functional surface can be defined (none of which, obviously, is the
anatomical surface of the LOT). The populations of the cortex share a common
functional surface, a common anatomical surface, and a common boundary, which
is determined by the distribution of axon terminals of the LOT (LeGros Clark,
1956, 1957, White, 1965; Heimer, 1968). E. EXPERIMENTAL INPUT-OUTPUT VARIABLES Although each population
performs all four transformations and sustains both wave and pulse activity, this
activity is not always detectable. Pulses can be recorded from
any type of cell in the olfactory system except the granule cells (G), because
they do not have axons and do not produce extracellularly detectable action
potentials. Waves can be detected only
for the bulbar granule cells (G) and for the superficial pyramidal cells (A)
(Freeman, 1970). This is due to the individual and collective geometries of the
neurons of the populations. The waves of potential which are recorded are the
result of extracellular spread and summation of dendritic current in a volume
conductor. This current is produced by the dendrites of large numbers of
neurons. Populations whose neurons have dendrites oriented radially with
respect to their cell bodies generate a closed or monopole field of potential,
which is low in amplitude and restricted to the anatomical limits of the cell
processes. Populations whose neurons tend toward axial symmetry generate dipole
fields, which are high in amplitude and extend for greater distances. In the
olfactory system the granule cells (G) and the superficial pyramidal cells (A)
have dendrites oriented on an axis perpendicular to the population surfaces, so
that their dipole fields greatly overshadow the monopole synaptic current fields
of the mitral-tufted cells (M, T) in the bulb and the granule cells (B) in the
cortex. The activity of a
population can be observed in two experimental conditions-during normal
unstimulated operation characterized by "spontaneous" or background
activity, and during electrical stimulation characterized by evoked activity.
The spontaneous activity for single neurons for the great majority of bulbar
and cortical neurons takes the form of seemingly random pulse trains (Freeman,
1968a). The mean pulse rate is designated P,. The expectation density or
autocovariance is usually nonoscillatory. Spontaneous wave activity, which is
the EEG (electroencephalogram) of the bulb or cortex, takes the form of a
randomly varying potential having one or more characteristic frequencies of
oscillation. The wave amplitude histogram almost always conforms to a Gaussian
curve. Mean pulse rate for the single neuron, P., is usually less than
one-fourth the characteristic frequency of the EEG. The other type of activity
is caused by single-shock electrical stimulation of the PON or the LOT. The
former superimposes a volley of action potentials onto the spontaneous or
background activity of the PON and activates the bulbar complex cartel
(P++P)(M++TG = G) orthodromically (in the normal direction of transmission).
The latter superimposes a volley of action potentials on the background
activity of the LOT which runs in two directions: orthodromically to the
cortex, where it activates the cortical complex cartel (A++AB = B) (C), and antidromically
(or in the reverse of normal direction of transmission) to the bulb, where it
activates the bulbar simple cartel (M++TG = G) but not the periglomerular
population (PTP). The resulting wave
responses of the granule cell populations (G = G) or of the superficial
pyramidal cell population (A++A) are known as evoked potentials, and the pulse
responses of single neurons (P, M, or TI A and B) are known as the induced unit
responses. Sets of evoked potentials and induced unit responses are averaged to
remove background activity. The results are known as the averaged evoked
potential (AEP) and the post-stimulus time (PST) histogram. These averaged
responses are the prime working data for mass analysis. To estimate a pulse density
distribution, spontaneous pulse trains or induced unit responses are sampled at
many points under the surface, but to estimate a wave density distribution, the
EEG's and evoked potentials are best taken from epicentral regions on the
surface to represent the activity of masses determined from the spatial
distribution of the dipole fields. F. THE ACTIVITY DENSITY FUNCTION (A.D.F.) To recapitulate, the
assumption is made that a neural activity distribution exists across an
interactive neural mass, for which the magnitude of each point in the surface
is an activity density. The activity density is reflected in the pulse trains
and EEG waves recorded at and about each point. The next question is: How can
one determine the relationship between the observable events and the underlying
events of interest? In answer, it is inferred
that the activity distribution arises from the cooperative interactions of many
neurons by means of their functional interconnections. It is feasible to
describe the known dynamic characteristics of single neurons by means of
nonlinear differential equations an further, to describe the proposed
topologies of their massive interconnections in their surface dimensions by the
same means. The equations can be solved for initial conditions corresponding to
known or suspected input to a neural mass. The solution takes the form of an
equation for a time-varying surface, which is an activity density function
(a.d.f.), and which describes the neural activity distribution. Next, the time
dependent value of the a.d.f. at each point in the surface can be transformed
in accordance with the predicted relation between the neural activity density
and an observable event such as a pulse train or EEG wave. The comparison of
observed waveforms with predicted waveforms derived from the a.d.f. determines
whether the image constructed for the neural activity distribution is
admissible. Such comparisons are demonstrated in Figs. 3-6. The a.d.f. is defined only
for aggregates and populations or parts of them, and it holds only for
aggregates when continuity of function is imposed in both spatial and temporal
dimensions by the input. The dynamic properties of simple and complex cartels
must be predicted by generating an a.d.f. for each population and aggregate
constituting them. This is because the observable events and averages of them
are manifestations of activity by single neurons or groups of neurons of the
same kind, which is in the definition of the population. The observable outputs
of the olfactory bulb, which contains a complex cartel, are action potentials
from neurons in two populations, EEG waves from another, and action potentials
from the LOT, which is the real output to the cortex producing cortical
responses to bulbar events. The "output of the bulb" does not exist
except in relation to these observables, and in each case a complete set of
population a.d.f.'s is required for prediction and description. The concept here expressed
as the activity distribution of an aggregate or population is basically
equivalent to the representation of an active state by Sherrington (1929) for
pools of motorneurons in the spinal cord as the central excitatory state
(c.e.s.). The term a.d.f. is introduced in part to avoid the unnecessary
dichotomy between c.e.s. and c.i.s. (inhibitory) ; in part to avoid an
ambiguity in usage of the term c.e.s. between "state of excitation"
and "state of excitability" to the exclusion of the latter- and in
part to introduce explicitly the notion of a mathematical, continuously varying
surface density representing the activity distribution maintained by the
neurons in a population, which exists only as an attribute of the mass (see
Note 9). Distributions exist before
and after each transformation effected by a population. When the successive
transformations of the component neurons are lumped into four types (a
pulse-to-wave conversion, a wave-to-pulse conversion; a wave-to-wave
transformation; and a pulse-to-pulse transformation), there is a pulse a.d.f.
or wave a.d.f. to describe each input and each output. Further, a pulse a.d.f.
can be defined for each surface orthogonal to the trajectory of an axonal tract
or compound nerve. That is, an a.d.f. can be defined for cross sections of the
PON and LOT as well as for the olfactory mucosa, the olfactory bulb, and the
cortex. These a.d.f.'s are the state variables of the equations describing the
neural dynamics. The main task for
population analysis is to estimate the a.d.f. before and after each successive
trans formation, and from each pair to describe the essential nature of each
transformation. The method of analysis
proposed here and elsewhere (Freeman, 1963, 1972) for the dynamic properties of
neural masses logically parallels that used to analyze the Properties of nerve
axon and axon bundles. One side of the analysis involves the measurement of
observables. First, the electrical output of a population, when it is a
functional part of a cartel is measured in a broad range of conditions,
particularly in relation to behavior, so that a physiological dynamic range of
function can be defined for both "spontaneous" and evoked activity.
Second, by paired shock stimulation and the application of the superposition
principle a linear dynamic range is established. This range is extended by
piece-wise linear approximation, and the limits of nonlinear performance are
clearly defined. Third, within the linear and piece-wise linear ranges the
responses to electrical stimulation, AEP's, and PST histograms are fitted with
curves generated from the sum of exponential terms having both real and
imaginary coefficients. This constitutes the process of measurement (Freeman,
1972.). The other side of the analysis
is the formulation of mathematical equations describing three aspects of each
population functioning within a cartel-namely, the dynamics, the state of
activity, and the observed responses. First the dynamics of the mass are
described by a linear differential equation having state-dependent coefficients
formulated from the known or suspected physiological, pharmacological, and
anatomical properties of the cartel. Next, the distribution of neural activity
is described by an a.d.f., which is a solution to the differential equation for
boundary conditions corresponding to the stimulus conditions. Finally,
predicted waveforms are derived from the a.d.f. by transformations which are
specified by the rules relating the active states of neurons to the electrophysiological
events observed by recording, including the recording sites. The resulting transformed
a.d.f.'s (the predicted waveforms) are compared with the curves derived from
measurement (the observed wave forms). Congruence implies that the differential
equation is an admissible formulation of the dynamic properties of the cartel;
noncorrespondence gives reason for another try, either at observation or at
formulation. Because some of the coefficients in the differential equation can
be evaluated only by comparison with the measured response, mere congruence for
single responses is not difficult to achieve. It is more difficult to formulate
the differential equation, so that a change in one coefficient representing an
appropriate physiological parameter will change the solution to the equation in
the same way as a physiological change in the animal (or a change in the input)
will change the output of the cartel. A still greater challenge is to observe a
pattern of change in the cartel response, replicate it by changing a
coefficient in the equation, and verify by independent observations that the
necessary (predicted) condition or property exists in the cartel. As with membrane studies,
it is convenient at the outset to separate the time, distance, and amplitude
dependencies of the activity density, and to treat either static or
lumped-circuit properties in terms of ordinary differential equations.
Experimentally this is done by recording from central regions of active zones
in populations, and expressing the active state as a variable in time, t, x, y,
or V or (in the linear case) in the operator, s. Elimination of the spatial
dimensions reduces representation of the cartels to a flow diagram (Fig. 2).
The a.d.f.'s represented by the arrows are reduced to single-valued functions
of time and amplitude, and in this form they are the state variables of the
equations describing the dynamics of the cartels. G. SUMMARY It is assumed that
widespread configurations of neural activity exist in neural masses, which are
collective properties of the masses existing by virtue of dense connections
within and between masses. Masses can be conceptually isolated, defined, and
classified at four levels according to the complexity of their connections. The
simple, internally unconnected mass with common input is an aggregate. The
simple, densely interconnected mass with common input and common sign of output
(excitatory or inhibitory) is a population. An excitatory population densely
interactive with an inhibitory population forms a simple neural cartel. The
combination of an aggregate or population with a simple cartel forms a complex
cartel, which minimally suffices to represent observable masses in the
vertebrate brain. Cartels in series and parallel configurations form brain
systems. The distributed active
states sustained by cartels give rise to observable events such as pulse
trains, evoked potentials, and EEG waves. After appropriate averaging, those
observables can be treated as more or less direct representations of the
distributions of massive activity. The relation between observed and assumed
real events is established by representing the functional interconnections
within the masses by differential equations. Each solution to the equations for
specified input yields a theoretical or predicted activity configuration, which
is termed an activity density function (a.d.f.). The a.d.f.'s for a given
neural mass, whether in distributed or jumped circuit form, comprise the state
variables representing the active state of a neural mass under designated input
conditions. From the a.d.f.'s are calculated the predicted waveforms of
responses to specific inputs. The predicted waveforms are fitted to observed
waveforms to' evaluate the equations. IV. The Parameters of Neural Masses At the outset it is
feasible conceptually and experimentally to separate three independent
variables of the activity density function (a.d.f.). These are response
amplitude (described in Sections IV, A and B), time or frequency (Sections IV,C-E),
and space or the surface dimensions (Section IV,F-H). This results in three
sets of linear differential equations, each with its own set of coefficients,
which are gain coefficients, rate constants, and space constants. The
mathematics and essential numerical results of these approaches have been
summarized elsewhere (Freeman, 1967, 1968a,b, 1972). The present description is
restricted to qualitative aspects. The numerical evaluation of
interactive properties of neural masses, which are dependent on input and
output magnitudes, requires two types of gain coefficient. One is forward gain
and the other is feedback gain. Forward gain is a measure of the one-way
functional connection density between aggregates, populations, and cartels. It
can go from zero (when the formation of spontaneous action potentials is
suppressed by anesthesia or surgical trauma) up to the limit of the anatomical
connection density. Feedback gain is a measure of interconnection or the
functional interconnection density within populations and cartels. Since
aggregates have no anatomical interconnections, they never have feedback gain.
Thus, functional interconnection density within a neural mass is the defining
characteristic by which to distinguish a population or cartel from an
aggregate. It is the attribute most heavily responsible for the ways in which
the dynamics of a neural mass differ from the dynamics of the component
neurons. Therefore, the problems of quantitative measurement of this attribute
are fundamental in the study of neural masses. A. FORWARD GAIN The forward gain of neurons
in a mass can be treated as the product of two conversion factors. Each
describes one of the two conversions that are carried on by each neuron in the
aggregate or population. One is the conversion from. a pulse density function
for afferent axonal pulse trains to a wave function for dendritic current; the
other is the reciprocal conversion of dendritic current to efferent axonal
pulse trains. Therefore the forward gain is dimensionless, but the two stages
require reciprocal conversions of units-for example, from pulses/sec/unit area
Of neural mass (pps) at the input of the dendrites to amp/unit area of neural
mass (a) at the output of the dendrites, and from amp/unit area of the neural mass
(a) at the input of axons to pulses/sec/unit area of the neural mass (pps) at
the output of. the axons. The conversion factor of
each stage is given by the ratio of the instantaneous magnitudes of output to
input in units of a/pps and pps/a. In principle, all that is needed for
estimation of forward gain is the measurement of the population wave amplitude
over its range of variation and the concomitant pulse densities at the input
and output for the designated range. The former can be taken from a set of
instantaneous values for the amplitude of the EEG waves generated by a
population, provided the field geometry is correctly mapped. (The units of
measurement may be in <?>volts rather than amp, because the transverse
cortical resistivity in ohms-unit area is invariant.) The pulse densities can
be inferred from the- probability of pulse occurrence conditional (Parzen,
1960) on the amplitude of the EEG corresponding to the time of pulse occurrence
(Freeman, 1972). In practice the analysis is limited to those neurons for which
the requisite state variables are accessible. Moreover, recognition must be
given to the time delay between the occurrence of a pulse and its manifestation
in the wave, and vice versa. The delay appears constant for any neuron but varies
from one neuron to another. FIG. 3. The three lower frames show the
probability of pulse occurrence (triangles) for three single cells of types M,
A, and B, conditional on the time delay from the EEG wave maximum (+ 1s to + 3 s). The fitted curves are pulse probability waves. The
three upper frames show the pulse probability (triangles) for the same three
neurons, conditional on the amplitude of the EEG at the optimum time lag
determined from the frames below. The fitted curves are the input-output
conversion curves, from which forward gain and feedback gain are calculated.
Equations for the curves are given elsewhere (Freeman, 1972). The method for calculating
experimental conditional probabilities is as follows. A random pulse train is
sampled from a single cell simultaneously with the EEG for 1 to 5 minutes at
intervals of I msec. The amplitude range of the EEG is divided into 64 segments
or bins. For each value of amplitude as it occurs, the question is asked, Does
a pulse occur now or at any of the preceding or following 25 msec? Tabulations
are put into a table (64 X 51). For each block in the table the total number of
pulse occurrences is divided by the total number of wave amplitudes, each block
representing a certain time delay between the occurrence of pulse and a
particular wave amplitude. Each fraction is multiplied by 1000 to express it in
units of pps and is divided by the mean pulse rate, Po, to
express it as the normalized conditional pulse probability, P. The result is a
table of conditional probabilities, P(T The amplitude histogram
showing the distribution of the wave amplitudes is Gaussian, with the mean
equal to zero amplitude and the standard deviation equal to a. Whenever the EEG
is taken from the surface, negative values of potential are due to excitation
and positive values to inhibition of the neural populations generating the EEG.
The reverse is true for depth recordings. To surmount these sources of
confusion, the range of wave amplitude representing the increased excitation is
denoted "positive," and the range from + The conditional
probabilities in the table are averaged within these two amplitude ranges for A
.. in and A max at each value of T. Examples of graphs of the probabilities of
pulse occurrence conditional on the time delay from the amplitude maximum, P(Amax It has been found that each
individual mitral cell has its own characteristic time delay from the EEG, but
on the average it is one-fourth of a cycle at the on-going frequency. This is the
basis for proposing the existence of a. phase code by which this part of the
nervous system operates, about which more will be said later. Examples of graphs of the
probabilities of pulse occurrence conditional on the optimal time delay and the
wave amplitude values, P(A Three important
generalizations emerge from these input-output curves. First, the curves are
sigmoidal (s-shaped) ; thus it is seen that I the conversion factor is maximal
at zero wave amplitude and decreases exponentially as amplitude increases from
zero owing to excitation, or decreases from zero owing to inhibition. [The
graph for B (Fig. 3, right) must be rotated 90' counterclockwise for proper
viewing (Freeman, 1972). The reason is that both the theory and the
experimental method for determining the relation of pulse, P, to wave, V,
require that V be the independent variable, whereas the process of
pulse-to-wave conversion in dendrites requires that P be the independent variable.]
There are both upper and lower saturation levels for both conversions. Second,
for each stage the conversion is linear over a central range of input-output
amplitudes. Third, both sigmoidal curves have sharper curvature on the
inhibitory side. For dendritic pulse-to-wave
conversion the sigmoidal pattern is immediately intelligible in terms of the
ionic mechanisms of postsynaptic potentials of single neurons. For a given
ion-specific conductance change induced in the membrane by an afferent pulse
volley, the magnitude of the dendritic potential wave depends on the difference
between the existing membrane potential and the equilibrium potential of the
conductance, change. The magnitude of the second response superimposed on an
earlier response is smaller than the first, because the driving force is less
(Eccles, 1957). This inherent dependence of forward gain on response amplitude
can be described with a pair of first-order linear differential equations, the
solution to which yields the input-output curve shown in Fig. 3, right upper
frame. The same type of
amplitude-dependence of the conversion factor occurs at the sites of
wave-to-pulse conversion in neurons, the trigger zones. The typical wave-pulse
relation for a population (Fig. 3, left upper frame) is not identical to that
for a single neuron (e.g., Granit et al., 1963). The former is sigmoidal; the
latter is linear between threshold (zero firing rate) and a high maximal rate,
which can be sustained for brief periods of time but not indefinitely. There are two reasons for
the differences between these input-output curves for the population and the
single neuron. The asymptotic approach to zero of the foot of the population
curve can be attributed to a distribution of thresholds and of firing times
with respect to the population means among the neurons making up the
population. The asymptotic upper limit at three times the mean firing rate for
each neuron can be attributed to the requirement that the upper limit hold for
the neuron over an indefinitely long time-space, or (according to one form of
the ergodic hypothesis) over the entire population at any one time. That limit
clearly must be-lower than the upper limit on the maximal firing rate
accessible to a neuron firing in bursts over short time intervals. The operation of these two
population properties yields a sigmoids wave-to-pulse conversion curve which is
asymmetric. The curvature on the inhibitory side is twice that on the
excitatory side, and the range is half. The implication is that the
distribution of thresholds and firing times is directly related to or dependent
on the maximal population mean firing rate and the central mean, Po, so that
the single process occurring in membranes at the trigger zones (denoted by the
conversion coefficient) controls the sensitivity or gain for wave-to-pulse
conversion. This factor has not been identified at the cellular level, nor has
it been adequately described at the population level. On the other hand, there
are three processes in membranes at synapses controlling the factor for
pulse-to-wave conversion: the two ionic equilibrium potentials, which are
presumably fixed, and the conversion coefficient for the conversion process.
Like the conversion coefficient for wave-to-pulse conversion, it has not yet
been identified with known cellular mechanisms. The forward gain, being the
product of two conversion factors which are amplitude-dependent, is similarly
amplitude-dependent. Increasing activity in either direction from zero results
in decreasing forward gain. Transmission is
amplitude-limited normally at one of the two stages, activity at the other
stage thereby being restricted to the quasi-linear range, so that, unless
large-scale jumps in amplitude occur, the amplitude dependency of the forward
gain for the two stages in series is normally determined by one stage and by
one rate constant. Thus in the bulb the limits on wave-to-pulse conversion are
dominant, whereas in the cortex the reverse holds. B. FEEDBACK GAIN Dense interconnection
implies the possibility of feedback of activation (excitation or inhibition)
onto that part of a mass initially perturbed by an afferent volley. For this
reason analysis of the dynamics of interconnected neural masses requires
measurement of the parameter of feedback gain as well as forward gain. The level of interaction
within an excitatory population is "positive excitatory feedback
gain." That within an inhibitory population is "positive inhibitory
feedback gain." That between excitatory and inhibitory populations within
cartels is "negative feedback gain." (Interactions between cartels
cannot be "dense" within the meaning of the term used here. It is
likely that such interactions will best be described in terms of bias controls,
as defined below, but the level of experimental analysis has not yet been
advanced far enough to cope with this problem.) Within a population the
feedback gain is the square of the forward gain, and it is positive. Between
populations in a simple cartel the feedback gain is the product of the two
population forward gains, and it is negative. In all cases the gains are
dimensionless coefficients. Two methods have been
devised for estimating the values of negative feedback gain in simple cartels.
The first method is by taking the product of forward gains from measurements of
"spontaneous" activity. The necessary observed activity is accessible
for both conversion curves for the A population, so that the forward gain for
the A population has been calculated from the product of the derivatives
(Freeman, 1972). Because the average phase lag between the conditional pulse
probability waves for A and B neurons is about 90•, it is inferred that the
forward gain for the B population is equal on the average to that for the A
population. Therefore the feedback gain of the cartel formed by them is given
by the square of the forward gain for either. The second method is based on
measurements of AEP's in response to electrical stimuli varied over a range of
magnitudes, as described below. The close agreement in numerical values 'for
gain derived from these two independent sets of data and analysis on the
olfactory cortical cartel have provided a check on the validity of the methods
(Freeman, 1972). A population generates a
characteristic pattern of response for its neural activity distribution upon
impulse activation, depending on which level in the hierarchy it is functioning
in. The measurement of observed waveforms from each response pattern yields a
set of closed-loop rate constants or frequencies. These in turn serve to
evaluate the magnitudes of the feedback gain coefficients, which express the
mean densities of interaction within the neural mass. Experimentally it is
found that on the average the feedback gain coefficients are related as
follows. Feedback gain for the two kinds of population, the square of the
forward gain for each population, is denoted by KE and Ki, Negative feedback
gain, KN, in a simple cartel is equal to the square root of
their product, KN = (KEKI)1/2, provided the amplitude of the
response to a test signal exceeds the amplitude of the background activity. If
the test response is smaller than the background activity, then Kn =
(KgKiKo)1/3
, where Ko is a fixed
reference gain equal to the gain at zero wave amplitude (Freeman, 1967). The aggregate occupies a
position of special importance in the hierarchy, because it is to this
"open-loop" state that populations and cartels are reduced by very
deep anesthesia. When interactions are reduced to zero by this method, so is
the feedback gain, and the neural mass is a functional aggregate having only
forward gain, even though the anatomical connection density is unchanged. In
this state the open-loop rate constants of the component neurons, which express
the fixed time delays of the component neurons, become accessible to
measurement, Values obtained by measuring open-loop responses serve as the
basis for computing the values for closed-loop feedback gain from the rate
constants of the closed-loop responses. The essential mathematical
tool used for measuring these sets of rate constants and from them deriving
estimates of gain is linear systems analysis. The experimental and theoretical
bases and conditions for using this tool on neural masses have been described elsewhere
(Freeman, 19641 19671 1972). There are three main
difficulties in its use in the present context. First is the separation of
neural delays encountered outside a given neural mass (that is, those between
the input site for stimulation and the first junction point within the mass)
from those encountered inside the mass (those between the first junction point
for the re-entry of feedback in the closed-loop state and the site of output
for recording). For example, on electrical stimulation of the PON, the afferent
axonal delay of the path stimulated is part of the total delay measured of the
bulbar cartel Ç the open-loop state, but it does not occur within the bulbar loop when
the loop is closed. Second is the interpretation of the rate constants of the
neural mass in terms of the delays of the component neurons. Third is the
compilation of evidence that the open-loop rate constants do not vary when the
loop is closed or when the gain otherwise changes. These problems have been
considered in detail in the references cited above and in other work not yet
published. As experimental problems they are both challenging and intriguing,
and the proposed solutions strongly influence the details of the hypothesis of
neural masses here being outlined. However, the prior questions must be dealt
with here-whether the neural activity distribution of a population can exist in
a closed-loop mass as a continuum, and, if so, what its time course might be
when the population is functioning independently or as part of cartels of
increasing complexity. C. MONOTONIC RESPONSE PATTERNS OF
POPULATIONS The experimental results
show unequivocally that wave responses can be observed and measured as the
output of closed-loop masses. The excitatory population can be modeled as a
positive feedback loop, in which, for both forward and feedback limbs of the
loop, the response to an impulse input is a rapid rise in a.d.f. and a
monotonic fallback to the baseline level without an overshoot. (A ripple with a
frequency near 200 Hz may be observed to ride on the main transient.) The rate
of rise is more rapid and the decay rate is less rapid than the corresponding
rates for the open-loop response. The disparity increases with increasing
feedback gain KE. As closed-loop feedback gain increases from zero and
approaches the value of unity, the response decay rate approaches zero, which
implies that the closed-loop response to a pulse may last many hundreds of
milliseconds. Such long-lasting impulse responses are commonly observed in the
central nervous system. The same response
configuration holds for the inhibitory population with an important exception.
Whereas the a.d.f. of the forward limb increases on impulse excitation and then
monotonically decays, the a.d.f. of the feedback limb decreases and then
monotonically decays in mirror image to the a.d.f. of the forward limb. This is
because the subset of neurons initially excited inhibit the second subset,
which (being inhibited) disinhibit the first, which are free to be excited by
spontaneous activity. Thus, this pattern cannot occur unless there is sustained
or steady-state background activity of "bias" to the neurons in the
inhibitory population, against Which the inhibition of the feedback limb can
occur. This bias cannot be generated internally by mutually inhibitory neurons.
It must come from excitatory neurons, either as an excitatory aggregate or as
an excitatory population. In the olfactory system the
total removal of the olfactory receptors does not silence the activity in the
bulb or the cortex. Transection of the bulbar stalk silences the cortex but not
the bulb. Therefore, the bulb contains its own internal source of an excitatory
bias, which has been traced to the population of periglomerular neurons (P++P
in Fig. 2). The mechanism by which this
population is thought to operate is of crucial importance to the study of
neural masses, so it must be considered in some detail. The anatomical
connection density of these neurons is very high and must exceed the level
sufficient for unity gain. The neurons do not have inhibitory input, so the
only effective functional limits on their activity are the upper saturation
limits on their P-V and V-P input-output curves. Therefore, provided those
upper limits are sufficiently high to permit firing of each neuron at or above
some minimal mean rate, each pulse emitted by each neuron will in effect recur
upon that neuron (in smoothed form) with sufficient excitatory potential to
excite that neuron at a shorter mean interval than the preceding interval. In
effect, the closed-loop feedback gain can exceed unity, so that any random
event will cause the population a.d.f. to rise exponentially. But if the a.d.f. rises
above the running mean pulse rate (in the excitatory direction), the forward
gain of each limb of the loop diminishes, so that the feedback gain, which is
the product of the two forward gains, must decrease until it reaches unity.
Thereby the excitatory population is self -stabilizing in the absence of direct
inhibitory input. Any additional transient input that increases the a.d.f. must
further decrease the feed back gain and increase the decay rate of the impulse
response, so that the a.d.f. returns to the unity gain level of activity (Fig.
4, left). Conversely, any sudden decrease in the background level of excitatory
input from the aggregate of receptors must increase the gain above unity, so
that the impulse response increases with time, and the a.d.f. returns to the
level for unity gain. The periglomerular excitatory
population is the key to the study of neural masses in the olfactory system in
two major respects (as well as several minor ones). First, the dense
interaction at the local level provides a basis for the continuum of the a.d.f.
in the spatial domain, which the olfactory receptors in the aggregate cannot.
Second, its continuing output provides the excitatory steady-state bias
required by inhibitory populations and cartels for operation within linear and
quasi-linear ranges. So crucial is this set of
functions to the hypothesis being developed here that it cannot be expected to
apply to other systems in the brain, unless excitatory populations are found in
them as well. It seems likely that such populations will be identified for the
somesthetic system in the substantia gelatinosa Rolandi (Wall, 1962), which has
properties similar to those of the glomerular layer of the olfactory bulb.
Whether they occur in the retina, the cochlear ganglion, or some other stages
of the visual and auditory systems or in the spinocerebellar pathways is at
present unclear. The identification and functional analysis of excitatory
populations early in the transmission sequence of sensory systems is a major
unsolved problem in the development of a theory of neural masses. D. OSCILLATORY RESPONSES OF POPULATIONS
WITHIN CARTELS The existence of two
interconnected aggregates, one excitatory and the other inhibitory, is
conceivable and would be modeled as a simple negative feedback loop. The
response to an impulse input would be a simple damped sine wave. The frequency
of the oscillation would depend on the negative feedback gain; the higher the
gain, the greater the frequency and the slower would be the decay rate of the
"ringing." This arrangement has not
yet been found in the vertebrate brain. It would seem unlikely, because it
would require that each neuron be densely connected exclusively with the other
kind and not with its own kind, a pattern that could easily be identified and
has not been. Where negative feedback is found and properly measured, it occurs
between populations and not aggregates. Therefore the output, which is that of
each population functioning within a simple cartel, consists in two parts or
components: one (the dominant component) is the damped sine wave of the
negative feedback loop; the other (the minor component) is a monotonic or
nonoscillatory wave, which is the combined output of the two positive feedback
loops. The impulse response
(triangles) of the granule cell population within the bulbar complex cartel
(P++,P)(M++T G = G) is oscillatory (frames at right, solid curves). The
dominant oscillatory component (dashed curve) due to the negative feedback loop
is a damped sine wave. It is superimposed on the minor monotonic component
(dashed curve) due to the combined output of the two internal positive feedback
loops (M++T) and (G = G). These two loops are driven by the monotonic output of
the periglomerular population (P++P), as well as by the stimulus pulse. That is
why the minor component is upward. In the output from a simple cartel it is
downward. As the stimulus intensity
increases (from top frames to bottom) the observed waveform amplitude
(triangles) increases, and its shape changes. The rate coefficients of the
predicted waveform (fitted curves) also change. The changes can be accounted
for solely by the amplitude-dependent pattern of change in functional
interconnection densities (feedback gains) within the cartel, which is
predicted by the curves in Fig. 3 (upper frames). From Freeman (1970). FIG. 4. The impulse response (triangles) of
the periglomerular excitatory population (P++P) is monotonic (frames at left). The minor component for a
population in a simple cartel constitutes an "internal bias" on the
operation of the oscillatory mechanism, which has a profound effect on the
frequency and decay rate of the damped sine wave. Owing to the asymmetry of the
conversion curves of the component neurons (Fig. 3), the internal bias is
characteristically inhibitory. This is manifested by a downward monotonic
base-line shift superimposed on the initially upward damped sine wave response
to an impulse. The granule cell population functioning as part of the simple
bulbar cartel on antidromic invasion from the LOT typically generates this
waveform. The orthodromic response
through the PON is that of the granule cell population functioning as a part of
the bulbar complex cartel, because the input is cascaded through the
periglomerular population. The sustained excitatory input from this excitatory
population following on the initial input causes the population to display an
excitatory base-line shift; that is, the initially upward damped sine wave is
superimposed on an upward minor component (Fig. 4, right). This is the
manifestation of an "external bias" from the periglomerular
population operating into the cartel in parallel to the transmission line for
the original activating pulse. The significance of
external bias on simple cartels lies in frequency control. All neural
populations functioning separately and as part of cartels have in common the
amplitude-dependence of feedback gain. As the response amplitude of a
population increases above mean, the feedback gain decreases, with the result
that response frequency decreases and response decay rate increases. The effect
of internal inhibitory bias is to augment the sensitivity of frequency to
amplitude changes. The effect of external excitatory bias is to stabilize the
frequency and to augment the sensitivity of decay rate. Thereby in the typical
bell-shaped surface distribution of the bulbar focal response to PON
stimulation, the response amplitude at the center of the focus decays faster
than it does in the surround, but all parts decay at the same frequency irrespective
of initial amplitude. It will be seen shortly
that the oscillatory responses of populations in the bulbar cartel following
electrical stimulation of the PON or of the olfactory mucosa not only have a
common frequency of oscillation, typically between 40 and 80 Hz, but also have
a common phase after the first cycle, despite prolonged conduction delays in
activation across the surface. The external excitatory bias mechanism explains
the common frequency but not the common phase. That can result only from dense
interconnection among neurons in the cartel. E. FEEDBACK IN THE TWILIGHT ZONE The concept of pure
excitatory feedback in networks of neurons was familiar to nineteenth century
neurophysiologists and was described by Ramón y Cajal (1955, p. 11) with the
term "avalanche conduction"; by Ranson and Hinsey (1930) with the
term "reverberating circuit"; and by Lorente de Nó (1933) with the
phrase "closed self-exciting chain." Lorente de Nó (1938, p. 233)
later disavowed use of the last two terms to describe activity of
"internuncial" neurons, because they omitted explicit reference to
inhibitory actions. He also stated that his network diagrams based on Golgi
stains. were heuristic schematics and were not adequate to describe the
complexity of neural masses. Specifically he viewed the activity sustained
among "internuncials" in response to impulse activation as a
continuous barrage. The periodic firing of a motor neuron subjected to the
steady level of excitation was owing to the refractory period of the motor
neuron, he said (p. 241), and not to the circulation of a volley within the
mass. Nevertheless the term "reverberatory discharge" (Hebb, 1949)
has remained in general use for the concept of circulating impulses as the
basis of short-term memory. The tenacity of the
concept, in the absence of any valid physiological evidence for it, is highly
significant. Reverberation is the only basis on which the pulse-logic
hypothesis can deal with excitatory feedback in networks. The experimental fact
is that regularly periodic firing is characteristic of isolated neurons or
injured neurons. It is progressively less common as the number of neurons in a
mass increases. Among mammalian central neurons, periodic firing is the
exception and not the rule. This does not preclude the possibility that such
neurons are part of fixed circuits required by learning theory, which might
have variable circuit times, but the analysis becomes cloudy. Therefore, while
pulse-logic analysis is effective for describing forward transmission, it fails
on application to feedback, primarily because recurrent neural action with few
exceptions is a mass action, and as such it is continuous and not impulsive. The difficulty has not lain
in the lack of adequate mathematical formalisms to handle discrete-valued
feedback system. It has stemmed from the experimental fact that feedback in a
neural mass cannot be reduced to a discrete-valued variable. Two syllogisms
follow. If the brain is a pulse-logic device, then local feedback is a trivial
part of its operation, and continuous events such as evoked potentials are
second-order epiphenomena. If local feedback is essential, then theories of
neural function must provide a continuum as the basis for measurement of
feedback gain, using wave events such as AEP's and PST histograms as the raw
data. Failure to comprehend this
dilemma has led some neurophysiologists into a twilight zone, in which
feedback, especially negative feedback, is accepted as an experimental fact,
but it is considered to occur at such low gain that responses are overdamped. The empirical evidence of
this 'failure can be found in most electrophysiological studies of complex
mechanisms such as the spinal cord (Eccles, 1957) ; the cerebellum (Eccles et
al., 1967) ; the visual cortex (Bartley, 1959) ; the hippocampus (Kandel and
Spencer, 1961); and the olfactory bulb (Phillips et al., 1963; Shepherd, 1963).
Typically the effect on these structures of anesthesia, surgical isolation, or
prolonged drug-induced immobilization of the body is the reduction in
complexity of interactions among neurons (feedback gain) and the enhancement of
forward transmission (forward gain), which can be studied one synaptic layer at
a time. Typically the response of
neurons in an obtunded mass consists in an initial burst of excitation followed
by a prolonged inhibitory overshoot, especially if the input intensity is high.
This is commonly regarded as due to recurrent inhibition and therefore as a
closed-loop response. In fact, it is only marginally so. Recurrent inhibition
in a neural mass is more characteristically directed by each neuron to others
in the mass than itself, so that true recursion would require more than the one
cycle of excitation followed by rebound inhibition. What happens is that input
is delivered along many parallel lines to many excitatory neurons. These excite
inhibitory neurons, which spread their effects widely. Any one excitatory
neuron therefore receives excitatory input from the afferent lines and
inhibitory input mainly from other afferent lines through the intervening
excitatory and inhibitory neurons. This is predominantly parallel feed-forward
inhibition and is closer to the open-loop than to the closed-loop case. There is a normal
physiological range of operation for the cortex and bulb. Stimulation near
threshold evokes activity having the same amplitude range and frequency range
as does the normal EEG activity evoked by odors. But, the evoked potentials
must be averaged to be seen over the background activity. This procedure yields
closed-loop responses having more than one cycle of excitation followed by
inhibition. The two reasons for the failure of normal responses to occur are
that anesthesia and surgical trauma depress background activity and thereby
remove or diminish normal operating bias, and that typically neurophysiologists
raise the stimulus intensity until the response waveform to a single shock
stands out over the residual background level. But this is a distorted
waveform. It no longer has the characteristic frequency of the EEG. Neural
responses of this overdamped type, which are the most common
electrophysiological response to artificial stimulation in surgical
preparations, are unsatisfactory as the basis for measuring either the open-loop
or the closed-loop rate constants. Closed-loop response
patterns have been measured in a number of complex systems such as Limulus eye
(Knight et al., 1970) and the hippocampus (Horowitz, 1972), when the logic of
the continuum has been used. F. THE DISTANCES OF MASSIVE INTERACTIONS The quantitative study of
dense functional interconnection requires answers to three questions: How
strong? of what duration? and over what distance? By far the most difficult to
answer experimentally is the third, because the input and output functions are
so inaccessible. It should be recalled that
the property of dense interconnection implies the convergence of input from
many neurons to each neuron, the divergence of output from each neuron to many
others, and the recurrence of that output through many others back to each
neuron. The output of the convergence operation is a pulse train (or its
equivalent) of one neuron in a mass, and the distances of convergence through
the mass can be measured easily by determining the surface dimensions over
which stimulation by the input function alters that point process. It might be
supposed that divergence is the simple inverse of convergence in geometric
terms. This is not usually true. In most systems it is not feasible to reduce
the input function to a pulse train of a single neuron or receptor. That input
is lost in the background activity of the neurons to which it is transmitted. Some neural masses exist,
as in the mammalian geniculocalcarine and lemniscal systems, for which the main
function appears to be the detection and amplification of activity in single
sensory neurons, or very small numbers of them. One can argue that the
amplification of activity in one sensory neuron by a factor of 101 or 104 to
the cooperative activity of that number of cells in a cortical column is
essential, before the neural event is thrown into the boiling turbulence of
distributed computation, and that the divergence in the cartels of these
systems is superbly adapted to the requirement for preservation of information
about precise spatial location. These systems also display a high degree of
topographic localization and are the best proving ground for the pulse-logic
hypothesis. But at and after the first
synapse in these systems the event must be considered multineuronal, and
attempts to map that divergence have not been successful (Armett et al., 1962).
In the vertebrate auditory and olfactory systems the organization of receptor
mechanisms is such that the activation of single receptors is not possible. The
impossibility of activating by sensory stimulation a single neuron selectively
is by no means incompatible with the pulse-logic hypothesis of neural function
(H. B. Barlow, 1969), but the blunt experimental fact is that the measurement
of divergence requires a distributed input function and not a point process. This constraint does not
apply to the evaluation of the anatomical basis for divergence. Sholl (1956),
for example, estimated the number of possible connections of the branches of
single neurons in cortex from the number of apparent fiber crossings in Golgi
preparations. He expressed the results in terms of the density of possible
connections as a function of distance from the cell body of each neuron. To
illustrate his results he showed plots for two stellate cells, which had
exponentially diminishing densities of fiber crossings with distances from the
somas. The length constants can be estimated as on the order of 35 to 50
microns. Such data as these are of great value for studies of neural
interactions, and it is astonishing that so few of them have been published.
They provide some essential structural constraints and guidelines to
physiological measurements. But they cannot supplant the dynamic measurement,
because the anatomical synaptic connection is necessary but not sufficient for
transmission to occur. On these grounds the study
of divergence demands that an input a.d.f. and an output a.d.f. each be
postulated, and that sampling be undertaken of neural activity across both
surfaces to validate the functions and evaluate the parameters of the
functions. The divergence is then expressed in terms of the relationship
between the two a.d.f.'s. The nature of the relationship depends on the nature
of the divergence. There are two basic classes of divergence, tractile and
synaptic, and for each class there are three kinds. G. TRACTILE DIVERGENCE Tractile divergence is
between cartels and is based on the geometric properties of neural surfaces,
compound nerves, nerve tracts, and their axonal terminals. The simplest form is
called dilative and is due to the change in the packing density-of a constant
number of axons, as from the cross section of a tract through which axons pass
to a neural surface on which they end. The second is called interspersive and
is the result of interweaving of axons in the manner of diffusing particles,
independently of any branching. The third is called collateral divergence and
results from repeated branching of an axon along its main trajectory or over
the surface at its terminals-that is, parallel or orthogonal to the main axis
of transmission. Neural fibers can be treated
as lines or curves with or without branching, and transmission of pulses can be
treated geometrically or analytically as the movement of particles or points.
In sufficient numbers they can be treated by using continuous approximations
related to diffusion or the heat equation. The key to proper design of the
equations lies in expression of the particular geometries of the axonal beds.
Each tract or nerve carries out a specific spatiotemporal transformation on its
input, depending on the nature of the tract geometry. This is why a detailed
catalog of nerve tract geographies is needed. The continuous curves used
thus far experimentally to describe divergence have been the exponential curve
(Sholl, 1956) and the normal density curve (Kirschfeld and Reichert, 1964;
Rodieck and Stone, 1965; R. Barlow, 1969) each requiring two parameters for
evaluation of distance from the center of the distribution in the surface.
Characteristically the distributions of neural activity observed experimentally
are less regular than these curves would imply. The use of the second curve
implies that the irregularities in observed distributions are chance departures
from the normal distribution. Essentially this is the null hypothesis. So
little is known about neural divergence that there is as yet no experimental
reason to reject it for describing the tractile divergence displayed by
responses to electrical stimulation. Examples of tractile
divergence can be taken from the olfactory system. Electrical stimulation of
the PON activates a distribution of axons around the stimulating
microelectrode, which can be fitted with the normal input density function with
the standard deviation, a,,, to represent the input a.d.f. of the PON in the
stimulus plane orthogonal to the PON axons. The value for sa
from measurements of the compound action potential across the distribution
averages about 200 microns. The surface dimensions of the volley which is
delivered by the PON axons to the surface of the bulb can also be expressed in
terms of normal density function with standard deviation, si, to
represent the output a.d.f. of the PON. The latter can be evaluated from
recordings of the evoked neural pulse trains and field potentials in the bulb.
The variance of the output a.d.f., si2, exceeds the variance of the input a.d.f., sa2, minimally by a factor of 11. This is equal to the ratio of the
cross-sectional area of the bulb on which the PON ends to the cross-sectional
area of the PON at the site of stimulation. Therefore, in this minimal case
where each a.d.f. is expressed as a normal density function, the divergence
operation is expressed by the multiplication of the variance of the input
function by the ratio of the two areas. This is dilative divergence. The overall divergence from
the receptors in the mucosa to the first passage through the mitral cells is
very large. Measurement of a,, in the PON implies that the 95% effective
stimulus radius of a microelectrode is about 0.5 min on the average. If this
holds for the mucosal surface for about the same stimulus intensities, the
region of activation encloses about 0.75 mm', or 0.2% of the receptors in the
1600 mm2 of mucosal surface. The zone of activation of bulbar
granule cells for this stimulation is estimated to cover between one'-sixth and
one-third of the 176 min' of the bulbar surface, from measurement of the
surface size of the bulbar AEP field. Most of this divergence is due to the
extensive reorganization and interspersion of axons between the mucosa and the
PON (LeGros Clark, 1956). In terms of the ratio of surface areas of mucosal
activation (0.75 mm2) to bulbar input (30 to 60 mm2),
the divergence is by a factor, P, of 40 to 80. In terms of the percentage of
neurons involved, the increase is from 0.2% of receptors to about 20% of bulbar
neurons, or by a factor of 100. If surface boundaries are ignored, this means
that the standard deviation of the activity of a normally distributed subset of
receptors is multiplied by six to ten times to give the standard deviation of
the subset of granule cells to which those receptors transmit through the
glomeruli and mitral cells. This statement stands in
stark contrast to the better-known "convergence ratio" for the
olfactory bulb, which for the cat is 140 million receptors to 80 thousand
mitral cells, or roughly 2000 to 1. Furthermore, each PON axon branches only
within a part of one glomerulus and makes contact maximally with only the few
dozen mitral cells having apical dendrites within that glomerulus; that is,
collateral divergence in the PON is negligible. The tractile divergence of the
PON becomes apparent only when the distribution of receptors in some local
domain of the input surface is mapped into the whole of the bulbar surface. Collateral divergence is
paramount in the LOT, because each mitral axon gives off repeated branches in
its trajectory across the cortex. These are strongly interspersive, so that
dilative divergence is negligible; the surface areas of the bulb and of the
cortex are approximately equal. The quantitative degree of
collateral divergence over the cortex has not been determined with adequate
precision. White (1965) commented concerning his histological findings:
"These observations indicate the absence of a point to point relationship
within the projection area of the olfactory bulb via the LOT and tend to
indicate that minimal activation of the bulb could readily activate the entire
cortex" (p. 473). On the other hand, Heimer (1969) wrote: "The
classical anatomical and physiological observations by LeGros Clark (1951) and
Adrian (1950), both suggesting a certain degree of spatial localization in the
primary olfactory projections, continue to excite our curiosity. Degeneration,
localized to certain parts of the lateral olfactory tract, has been observed in
animals with restricted lesions in the olfactory bulb in several pilot studies,
and the time seems ripe for a more serious attempt to unravel a possible
topographic organization within the secondary olfactory connections . . ."
(p. 144). These contrasting statements suggest that bulbocortical axonal
projections might also be modeled using the bivariate normal density function,
which would be continuous across the full extent of the surface, have a
-maximum density at one location on the surface, and approach zero with
increasing distance from the peak. A unique feature of
collateral divergence is the precise and reproducible delay in transmission of
pulses at terminals in the surface along the axis of transmission. The velocity
of a wave front would not in general be constant, because branching axons taper
and the conduction velocity of branches is lower than that of parent fibers,
but arrival times are sequential and fixed along the axis. Wave fronts also
occur for input by way of the PON, but because the divergence is interspersive
rather than collateral, the precision of succession cannot be as great. H. SYNAPTIC DIVERGENCE Divergence within cartels
(specific configurations of interactive populations) is by means of both pulse
propagation and electrotonic conduction. The former is characteristic of axons
and the latter of dendrites, but either may occur in both. Departures from this
rule are of no apparent importance in population analysis, because it is the
overall sequence of conversions that matters and not the number of trigger
zones in each neuron. Because sequential time delays are expressed in terms of
sequential synaptic transactions, it is logical to express the successive steps
of synaptic divergence by lumping together the axonal and dendritic components
of the spread at each stage. Corresponding to the three
types of feedback interaction, there are three kinds of synaptic divergence.
Excitatory divergence consists in the excitation of excitatory neurons by other
excitatory neurons. It is monotonic in distance across the surface, as it is in
time. Inhibitory divergence results when excited neurons inhibit their
neighbors and these disinhibit and thereby excite their neighbors in turn. The
pattern is monotonic in time but oscillatory in distance. Negative feedback
divergence occurs when excitatory neurons excite inhibitory neurons, which
inhibit other excitatory neurons; these in turn disexcite (inhibit) other
inhibitory neurons yet further removed from the initial focus of excitation.
The event is oscillatory in time, but it is monotonic in distance, because each
new subset of neurons is recruited in phase with the damped sine wave
established at the center of the focus. An apparent instance of
excitatory divergence is the spreading cortical depression of Leão (1944;
Grafstein, 1956), in which cell-to-cell transmission of excitation is the
mechanism. However, this is a pathological process and is not a good model for
normal interactions. As yet in the olfactory system no evidence has been found
for the occurrence of either excitatory or inhibitory divergence. The former
has been sought particularly among periglomerular neurons after PON electrical
stimulation, and without success. The reason appears to be the anatomical fact
that the length of cell process of periglomerular cells and especially of
granule cells in directions parallel to the. surface is small in comparison to
the dimensions of the input divergence. This can be shown as
follows. Suppose that the distribution of effective branches around each
periglomerular cell conforms on the average to a Gaussian distribution, with Divergence of evoked neural
activity does occur within the bulbar cartel by negative feedback. It is based
on the very long basal dendrites of mitral and tufted cells, which extend
parallel to the bulbar surface for distances up to 900 microns in all
directions. This is shown diagrammatically in Fig. 5, summarizing the results
of measurement of PST histograms of mitral-tufted units within an elliptical
focus activated by single-shock electrical stimulation of the PON on the
lateral surface of the bulb. The initially activated mitral cells (represented
by + symbols) lie within an ellipse, the area of which is determined by the
axonal divergence at the input. This area does not increase during the next
succeeding period of re-excitation of mitral and tufted neurons by their
interconnecting axon collaterals, which are relatively narrowly distributed
(White, 1965, p. 469). The diameter of the elliptical zone of granule cells
excited through reciprocal synapses on the mitral-tufted basal dendrites (Rall
and Shepherd, 1968) is calculated from the distribution of the initial surf
ace-negative peak of the surface AEP. It is larger than the zone of initially
excited mitral-tufted neurons, but smaller than a surrounding zone of initially
inhibited mitral-tufted neurons, which show inhibition at and after the crest
of the granule cell response. Fig. 5. The frame at lower left shows the
locations of mitral cells initially excited (+), inhibited (-), or not affected
(0) by single-shock electrical PON stimulation at the site indicated. The
insets above and at right show the surface distribution of potential of the AEP
across the center of the response focus at the crests of the first negative
peak (ND, the first positive peak (Pi, inverted), and the second negative peak This pattern of divergence
also can be described mathematically by the addition of variance. The several
successive regions of excitation or inhibition can be fitted each with the
normal density function having the measured standard deviation, This simple formula holds
through the establishment of inhibition among mitral-tufted cells surrounding
the initial zones of excitation. It predicts that, with each successive
quarter-cycle of the oscillatory interaction between mitral-tufted and granule
cells, another increment in variance will occur. Provided the initial input
variance is not excessive (and in many experiments it is not), this successive
widening should be readily observable in both PST histograms and surface AEP's.
After the first full cycle of oscillation, further radial spread has definitely
not been observed (Fig. 5). Once the oscillatory focus has been established, it
tends to remain as a standing wave within the confines of the focus. Therefore,
the simple addition of a variance is an inadequate representation for the
mechanism of negative feedback divergence. At present there is no
replacement. Metaphorically speaking, this is momentarily the end of the road,
and beyond this point there are only tenuous paths for further advance. I. SUMMARY Equations representing the
dynamics of neural masses can be formulated as sets of ordinary differential
equations in three independent variables-response amplitude, time, and space. Amplitude-dependency is
expressed in the form of two types of gain coefficients. Forward gain is the
numerical value for the functional connection density between aggregates,
populations, and cartels. It is measured from the instantaneous ratio of neural
output to input. The slopes of experimental and theoretical curves show that
the values for forward gain are functions of output magnitudes. Functional
interconnection density is expressed as feedback gain. For populations it is
the square of forward gain and is positive. For simple cartels it is the
product of the two forward gains and is negative. The time- and
space-dependent properties of neural masses are described by means of linear
equations with variable gain coefficients representing the nonlinearities. In
this formulation the open-loop rate coefficients are invariant- the closed-loop
rate coefficients, which are the coefficients of the a.d.f.'s for neural
masses, are amplitude-dependent. For an excitatory population the a.d.f. for
impulse input is monotonic in time and in space. For an inhibitory population
it is monotonic in time and oscillatory in space. For a simple cartel it is
oscillatory in time and monotonic in space. Beyond these general rules, the
temporal and spatial transformations effected by neural masses conform in
detail to the geometry and topology of the component neurons. V. Some Implications for Neural
Information Processing Three areas of observation
and analysis have now been examined, in which concepts from the doctrines of
centers and of pulse logic can be fused, so that cooperative and interactive
neural phenomena can be described. (1) Divergence is an open-ended process described
more appropriately in terms of a continuum than by means of discrete pulses for
the immense numbers of neurons and synaptic connections in vertebrate central
nervous systems. (2) Neural transmission involves alternating, conversions
between pulses and electrotonic currents. It is appropriate to, describe pulse
trains in terms of probability continua in order to relate them to synaptic
currents. (3) The dynamics of feedback, especially excitatory feedback in
populations, are most effectively described in terms of probability continua,
owing to the experimental fact that central neural pulse trains
characteristically are aperiodic. Most proponents of the
pulse-logic hypothesis have emphasized the "statistical character" of
neural events, but they have not devised criteria for determining the
spatiotemporal distributions, sample domains, or combinatorial laws for those
events. This is the most compelling reason to look beyond the data on single
cells and centers to the performance characteristics of great numbers of cells.
When one does so, the striking ,experimental fact is that neural masses have
properties that cannot be predicted from the properties of single neurons.
Among these are continuous waves of activity in time and space at varying frequencies
and amplitudes. The waves can be explained in terms of distributions of
single-neuron properties-for example, thresholds, connection distances,
synaptic delays (Rall, 1955; Ten Hoopen and Verveen, 1963; Calvin and Stevens,
1968), but because the distributions are properties of the masses, the waves
cannot be predicted from single-neuron properties alone. A. WHAT IS THE WAVE-PULSE DUALITY? FIG. 6. The expectation density (closely
related to the autocovariance), which is a property of the single neuron, is
nonoscillatory, whereas the pulse probability conditional on the amplitude and
phase of the EEG oscillates at the frequency of the EEG, which is a property of
the bulbar neural mass. The phase and modulation amplitude of the pulse
probability wave. P(Amax This conclusion is
illustrated in terms of the properties of single mitral cell pulse trains (Fig.
3, lower left, and Fig. 6). The typical, almost invariant pattern of firing is
nearly at random intervals at different mean rates averaging 10 pps or less.
The rates typically drop to half the mean between inspiration and increase two-
or threefold during each inspiration. The interval histograms conform to an
exponential curve with no intervals between pulses shorter than 2 or 3 msec.
The. expectation density (Gerstein and Kiang, 1960; Rodieck et al., 1962;
Poggio and Viernstein, 1964) of the trains rises exponentially from zero after
2 or 3 msec to a nonoscillatory level after the first 10 or 20 msec (Fig. 6,
upper frame) ; only those pulse trains having mean firing rates approaching the
EEG frequency at 40 pps or more show oscillatory autocovariance or expectation
density. The pulse-logic hypothesis
asserts from such data that each neuron signals the rate of airflow or the
concentration of some substance in the inspired air in terms of the numbers of
pulses occurring over some reasonable time span, perhaps 200 to 400 msec. A different picture emerges
from a graph of the pulse probability conditional on the amplitude and time (or
phase) of the EEG recorded at a site epicentral to the single cells. The
experimental curve, P(Amax The result implies that
each neuron in the readout population of a cartel transmits a wave as well as a
pulse. The wave is the sinusoidal oscillation in probability of pulse
occurrence at the characteristic frequency of the neural cartel. Being
determined by a phase (with respect to the mean phase for the mass given by the
EEG) and an amplitude at the momentary frequency characteristic of the mass,
the transmitted signal can be described as a vector. Owing to the property of
continuity of the a.d.f. over the local region around each neuron as well as in
time, the signal transmitted by the LOT can be described as a vector field in
the surface, either at the mitral cell layer or at the root of the LOT in cross
section, prior to operation of tractile divergence in the LOT, or in the
cortical surface after that operation. The vector for each axon
and the vector field for the LOT are properties of the cartel, because the
phase and amplitude are determinable only as functions of a signal of the cartel,
the EEG. The expectation density of pulse trains (Fig. 6, upper frame) does not
usually show the oscillation, because the frequency of the EEG and/or its phase
vary randomly in time, though they are the same at any one time across the
surface, and the pulse on the average occurs only once in every four or five
cycles of the EEG. Even when the mean pulse rate is fast enough to reveal an
oscillation, the phase cannot be determined from the expectation density or
from the autocovariance. This is the wave-pulse
duality. Certainly, the bulb transmits to the cortex only by means of pulses on
axons, but the neural mass hypothesis asserts that information is conveyed by
the pulses in the phase and amplitude of an oscillating probability wave of
pulse occurrence, having four or more times the frequency of the mean pulse
rate. The duality does not refer to the action potential per se. In physics,
for example, the wave-particle duality refers to an uncertainty regarding
position and energy level of an electron. There is no a posteriori uncertainty
about the time, location, or magnitude of an action potential. The duality
refers to the signal of a neuron, which is both a pulse and a wave (Fig. 6).
With respect to the single neuron it is a pulse, and with respect to the neural
mass it is a wave. In this duality the fusion
of the doctrines of centers and of pulse logic should now become clear.
According to the doctrine of masses, neural information is transmitted and
stored only in pulse trains (or the synaptic equivalent for granule cells)
generated by specific neurons at specific times and places (see Note 10). Many
neurons are required in parallel to transmit each message. The
"center" is represented by a cooperative domain having a common phase
reference. The EEG gives access to that phase reference, and by means of this
key to the pulse timing, the information in the pulses can be extracted. But
the EEG contains little or no information in itself. Neither waves nor pulses
can be read without the other. B. HOW MIGHT NEURAL VECTOR FIELDS BE
GENERATED AND RECEIVED? If it is known that a nerve
tract can transmit a vector field, then it is important to determine how a
sensory stimulus such as an odor might be encoded in that manner. This is a
difficult question in olfaction for two reasons. First, too little is known
about the nature of the olfactory receptors: whether there are basic types as
for color in vision, and, if so, how many (Amoore, 1971); whether their
locations with respect to airflow are important as for auditory receptors with
respect to distance from the oval window; what their pulse train
characteristics are, etc. Second, a mathematical description combining the
time-, amplitude-, and space-dependent properties of the bulbar and cortical
relays has not been developed. This will require the use of partial
differential equations, either quasi-linear with state-dependent gain
coefficients (Freeman, 1972) or nonlinear (Wilson and Cowan, 1971, 1972).
Without them quantitative prediction cannot be made with adequate precision for
experimental testing, while verbal descriptions tend to bog down in ambiguous
referents and double negatives. Even so, the main outlines
of the process can be sketched. It is widely accepted that olfactory receptors
differ among themselves in respect to sensitivity to different odors, and that
an odor drawn into the nose by a sniff elicits a unique spatial pattern (LeGros
Clark, 1951, 1957; Adrian, 1950, 1953) of excitation and inhibition among some
but not all receptors scattered over the mucosa .(see Note 11). The
anteroposterior rate of stimulus activation in the mucosa is presumably
determined by the rate of airflow and is about that of the mean conduction
velocity of the PON axons (Dravniecks, 1964; Freeman, 1969). An afferent surge
composed of action potentials and the absence of expected action potentials
leaves the mucosa during a sniff and undergoes temporal dispersion and spatial
transformation by tractile divergence. It is gated into the bulb by the
periglomerular population, which closes the gate by a process related to
presynaptic inhibition in the spinal cord (Wall, 1962; Eccles, 1964; Voronkov
and Gusel'nikova, 1968; Freeman, 1970) beginning 10 to 20 msec after the start
of the efferent surge and lasting several hundred milliseconds. This much is
obvious from records of pulse trains from mitral-tufted and periglomerular
cells and from AEP's. Also, through glomerular
synapses the PON axons activate mitral and tufted cells, which initiate
oscillatory wave activity in the bulbar cartel. The periglomerular population
serves to shut off the PON input at the end of the first half-cycle of the
oscillation (10 to 12 msec), so that continuing PON input does not cancel the
effect of that preceding. By this means the amplitude of oscillation in pulse
probability of each mitral cell is determined by the density of PON action
potentials in the leading edge of an afferent surge delivered to its
glomerulus. The phase of the
oscillation in pulse probability for each neuron (with respect to the surface
AEP as the reference signal) has three determinants. First, there is a
systematic delay in arrival times of PON action potentials from anterior to
posterior (the direction of conduction) amounting to about one half-cycle in
length. But, by the end of the first full cycle of oscillation, the AEP at all
points on the surface has the same phase and of course the same frequency of
oscillation. The mechanism is obscure, but it involves coupling of the neurons
in the bulbar cartel. It cannot be explained simply as an artifact of the
bulbar volume conductor. Second, the pulse-generating mitral-tufted neurons lie
in the forward limb of the negative feedback loop, whereas the field potential
generators (the granule cells) lie in the feedback limb, so on the average the
oscillation in firing probability leads the oscillation in field potential by
about 90º. These two factors determine the mean value for phase across the
mitral-tufted population. Third, the value of phase
for each neuron depends on the ratio of the forward gain to the feedback gain
in its part of the loop. These gains are determined by the amplitude of the
induced activity and by the bias levels in the forward and feedback limbs,
which determine the degrees of saturation in the two limbs. When saturation by
inhibitory bias is dominant in the forward limb of the cartel, Pm(AGmax The bias levels are
nonoscillatory in time, and the bias level within the excitatory population is
nonoscillatory with respect to the surface dimensions. But the bias level of
the inhibitory population is more complex, owing to the characteristics of
inhibitory divergence. Inhibitory neurons initially excited tend to remain so,
and those surrounding them ,hat are secondarily inhibited also tend to remain
so. The sustained differences in activity level tend to bias some parts of the
cartel in one direction and other parts in the other direction. This feature
implies that for focal inputs the bias levels within the cartel must vary with
distance across its surface, and so also must the value for the phase.
Specifically, in those areas of the bulbar surface in which granule cells are
initially excited, it is predicted that the mitral-tufted pulse probability
waves will show relative phase lead less than 90'. In those areas in which
granule cells are initially inhibited (disexcited), the mitral-tufted pulse
probability waves are expected to show phase lead more than 90º. With respect
to their own population mean (Fig. 7), initially excited mitral-tufted neurons
will have phase lead (light domains), whereas those initially inhibited will
have phase lag (dark domains). By this means the irregularities in the surface
amplitude distribution of PON input must lead to corresponding phase
irregularities in the vector field of the mitral cell output. Thereby an odor
can be encoded into a vector field. Readout of the vector field
in the LOT by cortical neurons is by vector summation of the pulse probability
waves, rather than by scalar summation of pulse trains as usually conceived.
The possibility of vector summation is predicated on the existence of a common
signal in the cortex, which is detectable in the cortical EEG (Boudreau and
Freeman, 1963). For transmission to occur from bulb to cortex, the two EEG's
must be coherent, and, in fact, a fluctuating and sometimes high degree of
coherence has been observed between bulbar and cortical EEG's in normally
behaving cats with implanted electrodes (Boudreau, 1964). With respect to the
common phase, each cortical neuron can be regarded as computing a running
Vectorial sum of input waves on its 101 input lines and generating a vector of
its own in the same code, irrespective of changes in the system carrier
frequency and amplitude, which are observed to undergo continual changes
(Freeman, 1963). C. DO TRAVELING WAVES EXIST IN NEURAL
CLASSES? FiG. 7. This is a predicted form of the
mitral-tufted population a.d.f. within the bulbar cartel on activation of two
glomeruli separated by 900 microns, in the presence of a traveling wave
condition. It is a phase interference pattern, in which light areas denote
phase lead and dark areas denote phase lag of mitral-tufted probability waves,
relative to the population mean phase. E, excitation; I, inhibition; 1, . . . ,
5, successive peaks of the AEP. Inset: a histological section through the
glomerular layer. Cresyl violet, 50X. In anesthetized cats the spread of
activity is not beyond I. Two kinds of traveling
waves are commonly known to occur in cortex. One is the spreading depression of
Leão (1944) for which a theoretical model has been developed by Beurle (1956),
and which is here classified as based on excitatory divergence. The other is
the spreading wave of dendritic potential upon direct cortical stimulation
first described by Chang (1951) as owing to axonal transmission delay, which is
here classified as based on tractile divergence, and which has been modeled by
Horowitz and Freeman (1966). Either or both of these mechanisms can be invoked
to account for the evoked and spontaneous spreading waves of cortical surface
potential first mapped in detail by Lilly and Cherry (1954). Neither of these processes
has yet been shown to play any role in neural information processing, and for
the olfactory system there is some evidence that neither does. On the contrary,
in the case of the bulbar response to electrical stimulation of the olfactory
mucosa or the PON, one is hard pressed to explain how a standing wave is
generated in the face of tractile divergence and excitatory divergence in
<?> input stages and synaptic divergence within the bulb. Part of the answer may be
that negative feedback divergence so greatly exceeds the excitatory divergence
within the periglomerular and mitral-tufted populations that Beurle's type of
wave cannot and in fact does not occur. The common occurrence of the
"inhibitory surround" phenomenon for units elsewhere in the nervous
system suggests that this negation holds rather generally. What cannot be
explained by intuitive logic is the discrepancy between (1) the fact that
synchrony in AEP oscillation develops within the first cycle across the whole
of an active focus, which implies widespread interaction among the bulbar
neurons, and (2) the fact that outward spread of the focus stops after the
first cycle (Fig. 5). The problem is nontrivial
for the following reason. These observations on spatial distributions were made
on anesthetized animals and with artificial stimuli. One can conceive a
mathematical model for the bulbar complex cartel, which has the stability
property of a high spatial damping factor in the presence of a low temporal
damping factor to account for the discrepancy described above. But suppose for
some combination of spatial and temporal damping factors in the model one were
to reduce the spatial damping factor. One can expect outward spread of the
focus in successive alternating steps of excitation and inhibition, as
suggested in Fig. 7. The successive steps are based on summation of variance as
described in Section IVA If two stimuli were delivered to glomeruli 900 microns
apart, a phase interference pattern would occur as indicated in less than 100
msec, owing to the superposition of alternating rings of initial excitation and
inhibition. The predicted wavelength between initial amplitude deflections
would average about 200 microns; the predicted wavelength between phase maxima
would average about 400 microns, because the alternating peaks of reinforcement
would themselves alternate between initially excitatory or inhibitory. By this
means the vector field of the bulbar output would acquire spatial periodicity,
as well as the temporal periodicity already demonstrated to exist. The
predicted periodicity is approximately two times that of the diameter of the
glomeruli in the bulb as shown in Fig. 7. It corresponds approximately to the
diameters of proposed cortical columns in the visual neocortex (Marshall and
Talbot, 1942; Hubel and Wiesel, 1963; Blakemore, 1970) and the somatosensory
cortex (Mountcastle, 1957). This configuration is the
neurophysiological counterpart of Lashley's prediction in 1942 "that for
any pattern of stimulation a stable resonance pattern, not unlike the
interference effects of simple wave motion, would be established" (p.
315). It has not been found in anesthetized animals. It is proposed that, if it
occurs, it will do so in waking animals, in which the activating stimuli also
evoke either an orienting reflex or some subsequently developed conditioned
reflex. Neurophysiologists will see
at once that this prediction will not be easy to test. The surface and depth
field potentials cannot be expected to reveal spatial periodicities having so
fine a grain. It will be necessary to record and process the pulse trains from
single neurons at ten or more sites simultaneously in waking animals, and the
ten sites must lie along a line (for example) in the mitral cell layer at
intervals of no more than 100 microns. Prior to such a prodigious
undertaking, it would seem prudent to carry out some preliminary studies. The
equations describing the bulbar cartel should be constructed, and the values of
parameters necessary for a traveling wave condition should be determined. These
values should be translated into the equivalent neurophysiological properties,
to determine whether it is reasonable to expect the traveling wave to exist.
Perhaps it will be possible to show that the necessary conditions can be
imposed in anesthetized animals by surgical or pharmacological manipulation of
the bulb (see Note 12). Tests of the bulbar AEP can be made by using repeated
single-shock stimulation to determine whether the surface field of potential
overlying a focus undergoes radial spread in the proper conditions. This is a
necessary event for the development of a phase interference pattern, and while
the pattern itself should not be and has not been detectable in surface AEP's,
the radial spread should be. Finally, further study must
be devoted to the properties of the LOI and the response configurations of
superficial pyramidal cells in the cortex, to determine in what conditions it
is possible for these systems, respectively, to transmit and receive a
vectorial interference pattern in the form of a phase front. The traveling wave
condition is not essential to the theory of neural masses, nor is it the only
prediction of its kind (Perkel and Bullock, 1968). However, it is immediately
relevant to many current questions concerning neural information processing
(Adey, 1966; Caianiello, 1967; Weirsma, 1967; Liebovic, 1969; Schmitt, 1970;
Campbell and Maffei, 1970; Pollen et al., 1969), (see Note 13) and it is
directly accessible to experimental disproof, so it appears at present to be a
promising avenue for further work. D. ARE NEURAL MASS ACTIONS
FIRST-ORDER, SECOND-ORDER, OR EPIPHENOMENAL EVENTS? The assertion has been made
that odors might be encoded in vector fields of neural pulse activity generated
in the bulb and transmitted to the cortex. This offers an alternative to the
assertion that odor is encoded in the discharge of a. small number of
characteristic neurons. But it does more. It offers the means for strengthening
the experimental investigation of the pulse-logic hypothesis in applications to
those systems, where it is most likely to continue to be the most effective
means for description. Even where rhythmic fluctuations in firing probability
of single neurons exist, they may be no more than the idling discharge of
immense numbers of neurons, each keeping its lines open for days, weeks, or
years by transmitting and receiving random test pulses, and each waiting for
some specific burst of pulses on two or more input lines signifying the
presence of its "psychon," upon which it will emit a burst attesting
to that fact. But which other neurons will read that burst? What are the
mechanisms for providing the background activity in millions of idling neurons?
What maintains each neuron in an optimal dynamic range? What are the burst
rates and coincidence intervals that distinguish a "signal" from a
random jump in background input? What are the a priori functional connection
densities, and how much greater than these must the effectiveness of each
single activated line become to qualify as a Hebb connection? These and related
questions refer to the mechanisms for establishing the "ground"
against which the "figure" of single-unit activity must be detected
or maintained. They are questions requiring population studies. Therefore,
neural masses are of interest not merely as phenomena in their own right, or as
the possible vectors of neural information in relation to behavior, but as an
essential aspect of the pulse logic hypothesis. On the other hand, there is
a peculiar skeletal dryness about strict forms of pulse logic, which explain
neural operations by discarding so much going on as merely epiphenomenal. Neural activity in lightly
or moderately anesthetized animals or in immobilized animals is relatively
quiet, regular, and even stately. It is most suitable for the analysis of the
basic topologies of connections, evaluation of fixed parameters, specification
of state variables and observables, identification of intrinsic and extensive
degrees of freedom, etc. In the waking animal the activity becomes more lively,
especially in the presence of sensory stimuli and motivating antecedents such
as food deprivation (Freeman, 1962). The activity comes dramatically to the
foreground when the sensory or electrical stimuli are directed into the
olfactory system, from which the recordings are being made (Freeman, 1960; Emery
and Freeman, 1969). Above all, when the animal is in a state of learning with
respect to the stimulus evoking the recorded activity (Freeman, 1968a), that
activity comes to seem cataclysmic. Great waves of potential roll off each
stimulus at wildly fluctuating frequencies as though immense numbers of neurons
in the masses of the telencephalon were brought to focus on the primary cortex.
Yet these waves are only the surface ripples of events within (Adey, 1966;
John, 1967). When the full scope of the neural event in learning is eventually
brought to view, it will be an awesome spectacle, even as it occurs in the
brain of a mouse learning to run a maze for food. Hypotheses which express this
phenomenon in terms of Pavlovian switching circuits and coincidence detectors
of pulse trains may logically be correct, but their rectitude is that of the
statement, "The brain is made of neurons." Of course that is true,
but it is not the whole truth. E. PROPOSITIONS FOR A THEORY OF NEURAL
MASSES This essay is intended to
be propaedeutic toward the formulation of a theory of neural masses. Such a
theory is not yet achievable, partly because observation at the requisite level
of sophistication has not been extended much past the olfactory system, and
partly because the theoretical investigation of the statistical properties of
single neurons is incomplete. As an additional step toward the organization of
a theory, a set of propositions is listed in logical sequence, but without the
structural tightness that will be required for a theory. 1. It is assumed that an active state can
exist across large numbers of neurons, which is represented by covariant ( not
necessarily cooperative ) activity of those neurons. 2. A "center" is an anatomical
domain of neurons maintaining a given active state. 3. The neural mass is the inclusive locus of
domains of possible active states of a certain kind. The kind of active state
is predicated on a certain kind of massive functional neural connection. 4. There are two elemental types of functional
connection. One is forward or one-way, and the other is reciprocal or two-way.
Their large scale numerical values are designated forward gain and feedback
gain. 5. The local magnitudes of connection are
expressed as functional connection density and functional interconnection
density. Each may range from zero up to the limit set by anatomical connection
density. High functional interconnection density confers the dynamic property
of superposition and the topologically derived properties of convergence,
divergence, and feedback. 6. There is a hierarchy of level of neural
masses based on the complexity of neural connections, ranging from the
internally unconnected mass to the whole brain, with a corresponding range in
complexity of cooperative neural activity. 7. The aggregate is a neural mass with common
input and zero functional interconnection density, even if anatomical
connections exist within it. Its parameters are forward gain, a set of rate
constants, and a set of space constants. 8. The population is a set of densely
interconnected neurons with common input and the same sign of output. It is
represented by a positive feedback loop. In addition to the parameters of an
aggregate it has feedback gain. There are two kinds of populations: excitatory
having positive excitatory feedback gain, and inhibitory having positive
inhibitory feedback gain. 9. It is feasible to separate conceptually
and experimentally three sets of independent variables for population function:
amplitude, time, and space. Owing to the superposition property the
transformations of populations are susceptible to description using ordinary
linear differential equations in each of these dimensions. 10. Each population (and aggregate, if the
input imposes covariation) maintains a continuous activity distribution, which
has at each point in time and space a certain density. The solution to the
differential equations for the population dynamics is an activity density
function (a.d.f.) . There are as many a.d.f.'s for each population as there are
transformations by the population. Any one or two of these can serve as the
state variable of the population. 11. The activity density at each point is
reflected in pulse trains and field potentials. After appropriate
spatiotemporal averaging, the latter became the observed waveforms. Following
appropriate transformation the a.d.f. gives rise to predicted waveforms.
Parameters in the differential equations are evaluated by fitting predicted
waveforms to observed waveforms. 12. The amplitude conversions of the a.d.f.
are from waves to pulses (V-P) and from pulses to waves (P-V). Input-output
curves (derived from properties of single neurons) are sigmoidal but
asymmetric, with sharper curvature on the inhibitory side. 13. The product of the derivatives of the two
input-output curves equals the population forward gain. It is
amplitude-dependent. 14. The population open-loop rate constants
(measured by using models derived from properties of single neurons in the
aggregate) are invariant. The same values serve to describe the five
populations in the olfactory system. 15. The population closed-loop rate constants
are gain- and amplitude dependent. The impulse response is monotonic in time,
with decay rate approaching zero as feedback gain approaches unity. 16. The simple cartel results from the dense
interconnection of an excitatory with an inhibitory population. The active
state of a cartel cannot be expressed in a single a.d.f., state variable or
predicted waveform. There is at least one for each component population. 17. The impulse responses of populations in
cartels oscillate with an average quarter-cycle phase lead between the a.d.f.'s
of the forward and feedback limbs. The phase, frequency, and decay rate of the
oscillation are gain- and amplitude-dependent, both on the negative feedback
gain and on the two types of positive feedback gain. 18. In general, the positive inhibitory
feedback loop in the cartel contributes internal inhibitory bias and at unity
gain determines the stability characteristics of the cartel. A source of
external excitatory bias is required for normal operation. 19. In the olfactory bulb, the cascade of an
excitatory population into a simple cartel forms a complex cartel. The
population contributes normal de operating bias to the assembly, frequency
stabilization against changes induced by variation of amplitude, and an input
gate. The a.d.f. conversions are dominated by the limits on (V-P) conversion. 20. In the olfactory cortex a complex cartel
is formed by the cascade of a simple cartel into an aggregate. It displays
powerful external inhibitory bias, possibly because the limits on (P-V)
conversion seem to dominate the a.d.f. conversions. This stabilizes the decay
rate of oscillatory impulse responses, at the expense of strong variation in
frequency and phase with response amplitude. 21. Divergence between cartels is tractile
and takes three forms dilative, interspersive, and collateral-depending on the
geometric patterns of the axons. 22. Both cortical and bulbar cartels have
prominent tractile divergence in their input pathways. The open-loop space
constants of cartels are independent of electrical stimulus intensity and
response amplitude. In this they resemble the open-loop rate constants (cf.
proposition 14). 23. Divergence within cartels is synaptic.
The three types are cognate with the three types of feedback. For both
excitatory divergence and negative feedback divergence the response
configurations are monotonic in space. For inhibitory divergence the response configuration
is oscillatory in space, even though it is monotonic in time (cf. proposition
15). 24. The bulbar cartel generates a standing
wave field potential on impulse activation; that is, the phase and frequency
are everywhere the same. 25. The pulse probability of single bulbar
neurons conditional on the phase and amplitude of the field potential of the
cartel has the same .,,Sinusoidal frequency of oscillation as the field
potential, but the modulation amplitude and phase vary from neuron to neuron. 26. The cortical cartel generates a traveling
wave field of potential at a frequency determined by the cortical cartel and a
phase determined by the tractile divergence of the input tract, the LOT. 27. The pulse probability of single cortical
neurons conditional on the phase and amplitude of the cortical field potential
likewise oscillates at the common frequency but with variable phase and
modulation amplitude. 28. It is concluded that each LOT axon
transmits a wave as well as a pulse train, which is the sinusoidally
oscillating probability of pulse occurrence. The phase and modulation amplitude
of the wave convey a signal, which can be expressed as a two-dimensional vector
for that axon. 29. It is proposed that olfactory information
can be encoded in the form of a vector field established and maintained in the
surface of the bulbar cartel by the population of mitral cells. 30. It is predicted that in animals oriented
to an olfactory stimulus (chemical or electrical), a traveling wave condition
will be found, such that the bulbar vector field will have the configuration of
a phase interference pattern. VI. Summary From 1784 to 1940,
coinciding approximately with advances in the technology of the gross
electrode, the operation of the nervous system was conceived in terms of
"nerve energies" or "central excitatory states," which were
sustained by pools of nerve tissue forming networks and were related to
specific forms of behavior. Since 1940 and the advent of microelectrode
technology, conceptually the single neuron has replaced the "center"
as the element of neural coding, and the nerve impulse has replaced the c.e.s.
as the carrier of neural information in networks. It is proposed that with the
advent of computer technology in electrophysiology it is possible to fuse these
two systems of analysis. The new tools make it possible for the first time to
"see" certain neural events occurring as continuous spatiotemporal
distributions of nerve impulses or "waves," which reflect widespread
cooperative activity among neurons in "masses." The neural
information constituting the particulars of these events is conveyed only in
pulse trains of many single neurons, but that information is observable only
with respect to certain running averages or moments of activity across great
masses. In selected cases these collective properties are accessible by
recording the electroencephalogram (EEG). The information then appears in the
form of pulse probability waves transmitted by single axons at the frequency of
the EEG. Such cooperative activity
is conceived as arising only by virtue of certain forms of dense neural
connection. Rules are given here for defining neural masses in relation to a
hierarchy of complexity in connections, ranging from the "aggregate"
(with no internal connections) through the excitatory or inhibitory
"population" (having dense interconnections), to the neural
"cartel" (consisting of densely interconnected populations of both
kinds), and then to the familiar modality-specific neural systems (consisting
of networks of cartels). Each level of organization can be recognized by
characteristic response configurations, and each can be described by an
appropriate set of differential equations. Such equations are in one of the
three independent variables (amplitude, time, and space), and they have one of
the corresponding three sets of parameters <?> (multitude, time, and
space), and they have one of the corresponding three sets of parameters (gain,
rate, and space coefficients). The solutions for specified input conditions are
shown to yield the state variables, as well as predicted response waveforms.
The comparison of predicted and observed response waveforms provides the basis
for evaluation of the parameters and for provisional acceptance or rejection of
proposed explanatory equations. On the basis of results thus far, specific
predictions are made concerning the manner in which the bulb encodes olfactory
information. NOTES 1. Albrecht von Haller, the founder of modern
physiology, used (1776, p. 320) the term vis nervosae to describe an agent that
forced nerve fluid from the brain to the muscles to serve as a trigger for the
local release of muscle force (vis insita). He subscribed to the
Aristotelian-Galenic-Cartesian view of the brain as the seat or source of nerve
fluid, of neural activity, and of behavior. For Jiri Prochaska (1749-1820), a
professor of "morbid anatomy of the eye" in Prague, this tradition
broke with dramatic force, when he observed the behavior of anencephalic
monsters as reported in 1784. How (p. 188) could a human infant without a brain
be capable of movement? All parts of the nervous system including the spinal
cord and nerves must have intrinsic nerve force (pp. 51, 78). He subdivided the
lower nervous system into three parts: sensory systems, motor systems, and,
intervening, the common sensorium in the medulla oblongata, in which nerve
force released by sensory stimulation could be reflected suddenly and violently
("Subito et violenter" -p. 52) into the motor nerves. He conceived
the operation of this nerve force in Newtonian mechanistic terms (p. 29) and
thereby introduced the concepts of homeostatic and protective reflexes (p.
116): "The reflexion of sensorial into motor impressions, which takes
place in the sensorium commune, is not performed according to mere physical
laws, where the angle of reflexion is equal to the angle of incidence, and
where the reaction is equal to the action; but that reflexion follows according
to certain laws, writ, as it were, by nature on the medullary pulp of the
sensorium, which laws we are able to know from their effects only, and in
nowise to find out by our reason" (Prochaska, 1784, translated by T.
Laycock, 1851, p. 430). Prochaska was a mechanist
in a time when vitalism was both rampant and politically prudent. But by 1812,
following the discoveries of Galvani and Volta, he had thoroughly confused
"nerve force" with "animal electric tension" (Prochaska,
1812). In so doing he committed the same error as Köhler (1940), who identified
perceptual fields with electrical fields of current in the cortex (see Note
10), and as those investigators at present who would identify the
"waves" of neural activity here being discussed with EEG waves. 2. Sherrington (1925) made an explicit
distinction between the dependence of response magnitude on the number of
active neurons and on the degree of activity of each: "From the standpoint
which the diagram illustrates, reflex actions offer two discriminable
attributes, quantity and intensity, quantity being expressed as the number of
neurones engaged, i.e. the number activated in excitatory reflexes, and the
number inhibited in inhibitory reflexes; intensity, on the other hand, being the
excess of supraliminal state or the 'surcharge' exerted from up-stream on the
individual downstream neurons and 'all-or-none' mechanism, whether that
'surcharge' be excitatory, or inhibitional, or, as usually, some algebraical
resultant of the two together" (p. 542). He proceeded almost
apologetically to describe an "agent" (subsequently in 1929 named
central excitatory state, abbreviated c.e.s.) for this dependence:
"Reverting to the schema above proposed, one weakness lies admittedly in
its assumption of the existence of an agent simply as inferred from reactions
of which that agent could be the cause; and that agent, moreover, one, whose
existence lies outside the intrinsic properties of pure nerve-fibre and with a,
so to say, more chemical mode of origin and function than the nerve impulse per
se" (pp. 542-543). However, he was not
explicit as to whether the "agent" was a property of each neuron, or
of the population of neurons, or of both. Lorente de Nó (1938) identified the
"agent" with swarms of neural pulses: "Internuncial bombardment
. . . has all the properties of c.e.s. . . . and since a motoneuron despite any
possible lowering of threshold due to previous, intrinsic or extrinsic activity
does not fire unless impulses are delivered to its synapses . . . there is no
doubt that the central excitatory state' leading to, motor discharge is due to
internuncial activity and bombardment. Furthermore, it has been shown in this
paper that subliminal c.es., i.e., excitation demonstrable only by its ability
to facilitate the response to an intercurrent stimulus, is always accompanied
by internuncial bombardment, which under the conditions of the present
experiments overshadows the effect of any other factor capable of lowering the
threshold of the neurons" (p. 227). Electrophysiologists with few
exceptions after the development of intracellular recording identified the
"agent" solely with the single neuron in the form of the dendritic
postsynaptic potential. Granit (1967) wrote: "In his intracellular work on
the postsynaptic excitatory and inhibitory potentials in the spinal cord Eccles
may be said to have run out the course that Sherrington set, beside adding many
new and significant observations to those briefly reviewed. The concepts of
Sherrington (Chapter 3) have now been verified and reformulated in terms which
for postsynaptic excitation and inhibition agree almost word for word with
those of the old Master's, as was pointed out by Sir John Eccles himself in a
review of Development of Ideas on the Synapse (1959). [Cf. Eccles, 1964.1
Postsynaptic inhibition is a stabilization of the potential of the cell
membrane; it can be neutralized by postsynaptic excitation, the two processes
being represented by potential changes of opposite direction; both events can
be subliminal from the point of view of the firing mechanism and both are
capable of being nicely graded; specific sensitivity to either of the two is
likely to be localized in the cellular membrane rather than at the terminals
themselves, etc." Yet the level of
postsynaptic potential (PSP) of one cell (Eccles, 1964) is not the same as the
c.e.s. or c.i.s. of many cells, nor are their pulses; each is part of the
mechanism. The questions for population analysis are: How do the PSPs of single
cells relate to their active states? how do the active states sum to give the
c.e.s.? and how does the c.e.s. relate to some measured quantity such as pulse
firing or muscle tension? 3. Adrian (1947) wrote: "There would be
no point in discussing the artificially produced nerve impulse ... if it were
not reasonably certain that impulses of the same kind are the basis of all
nervous communication" (p. 12). Fulton (1949) wrote: "Since an end
organ must convey its messages to the nervous system by the all-or-nothing nerve
impulse, the only possible way of communicating differing intensities of
stimulation from a single end organ lies in differing rates of discharge"
(P. 12). Eccles (1952) wrote: "We may say that all 'information' is
conveyed in the nervous system in the form of coded arrangements of nerve
impulses" (P. 1). The march of technology hag
been crucial in this historical sequence. The macroelectrode made it possible
to observe and manipulate the neural "center." The microelectrode
likewise made the neuron and its pulse accessible. It is computer technology
that has opened the neural mass and its wave properties to observation. 4. Hebb (1949) incorporated Lashley's (1929)
data in his imaginative and very influential theory, -but his critique of
Lashley's concepts of mass action contained an equivocal paraphrase of
Lashley's position on stimulus equivalence. According to Lashley (1942):
"The principle involved is that the reaction is determined by relations
subsisting within the stimulus complex and not by association of a reaction
with any definite group of cells.... An indefinite number of combinations of
retinal cells and afferent paths are equivalent in perception and in the
reactions which they produce.... Here is the dilemma. Nerve impulses are
transmitted over definite, restricted paths in the sensory and motor nerves,
and in the central nervous system from cell to cell through definite
intercellular connections. Yet all behavior seems to be determined by masses of
excitation, by the form or relation, or proportions of excitation within
general fields of activity, without regard to particular nerve cells. It is the
pattern and not the element that counts" (pp. 304-306). Hebb (1949)
stated: "'Equivalence of stimuli has a double reference. It may mean only
(1) that different stimuli can arouse the same response. This is an observed
fact of behavior, whatever one's interpretation of the fact. But Lashley has
also used the term to mean (2) that it does not matter what sensory cells are
excited in order to get a certain response . . ." (p. 39). This Lashley
did not say. Further, "Lashley has concluded that a learned discrimination
is not based on the excitation of any particular neural cells. It is supposed
to be determined solely by the pattern, or shape, of the sensory excitation. .
. . This suggests that the mnemonic trace, the neural change that is induced by
experience and constitutes 'memory', is not a change of structure. . . . If it
is really unimportant in what tissues a sensory excitation takes place, one finds
it hard to understand how repeated sensations can reinforce one another, with
the lasting effect we call learning or memory. It might be supposed that the
mnemonic trace is a lasting pattern of reverberatory activity without fixed
locus, like some cloud formations or an eddy in a millpond . . ." (p. 12).
Hebb acknowledged the effort made by Lashley to reconcile the mass action
hypothesis with the necessity for specific structural synaptic changes as the
basis for learning: "Lashley's (1942) hypothesis of interference patterns
is the one explicit attempt to solve this difficulty and to deal adequately
with both perception and learning. As such it deserves special mention here,
although we shall see that in other respects it faces great difficulties"
(p. 15). But the "other respects" of the difficulties stemmed from
behavioral data, which were equally consistent with both Hebb's and Lashley's
premises and conclusions, and Hebb concluded merely that "the fundamental
difficulty with configuration theory, broadly speaking, is that it leaves too
little room for the factor of experience" (p. 58). In so doing Hebb failed
to meet Lashley's argument. Hebb's development of his
own discrete network hypothesis was inconsistent on precisely those points here
at issue. His "neurophysiological postulate" (pp. 62-69) was
developed in terms of networks of single neurons. In describing his synaptic
hypothesis he stated: ". . . structural connections are postulated between
single cells, but single cells are not effective units of transmission, and
such connections would be only one factor determining the direction of
transmission . . ." (p. 61). He stated the inference: "When impulses
in one such path are not effective, those in another, arriving at a different
time, could be" (p. 76). Thus he began. Eventually the cell
assembly became not merely "irregular" but "diffuse" (p.
86) and capable of sustaining "liminal or subliminal excitation" (pp.
86-87)-that is, a graded and continuous response variable. However: "At
each synapse there must be a considerable dispersion in the time of arrival of
impulses, and in each individual fiber a constant variation of responsiveness;
and one could never predict a determinate pattern of action in any small
segment of the system. In the larger system, however, a statistical constancy
might be quite predictable" (p. 76). Hebb stated concerning his graphic
models that "such neat connections" in a "three-dimensional
lattice" (P. 72) of neurons forming an assembly were "of course
statistical: the neurons diagrammed were those which happen to have such
connections, and, given a large enough population of connecting fibers
distributed at random, the improbable connection must become quite frequent, in
absolute numbers" (p. 74). The concept of uncertainty
about the location of a specific effective connection in a random set of
connections is not compatible with the concept of the continuous
("diffuse") distribution of effectiveness across the same set.
Clearly Hebb did not weaken Lashley's case. On the other hand, there was no
neurophysiological evidence put forward for "interference patterns"
by Lashley (1942). He seems to have been the T. S. Eliot of his generation of
neuropsychologists, and his writings often seem as bleak as "The
Wasteland." 5. Sherrington's model was not unique, though
it has stood alone in terms of the extent of its neurophysiological
documentation. Several other mass action theories of the nervous system have
been proposed, based on behavioral data, such as the spatial properties of
pattern perception (Köhler, 1940), or the effects of brain lesions on learned
behavior (Lashley, 1929), or on neuroanatomically based modeling (Cragg and
Temperly, 1954; Beurle, 1956; Sholl, 19%; von Neumann, 1958). This evidence has
been reviewed and up-dated recently, with the addition of new evidence based on
electrical recording of field potentials in the brain (John, 1967) and on
Skinnerian techniques (Pribram, 1969, 1971). These anatomical and
behaviorally derived data did not address or answer the question being raised
here, concerning the nature of neural mass action, because they did not yield
estimates of functional interconnection density. Suppose that the performance
of a given task such as maze-running were shown by destructive lesions to
depend on the quantity of a certain mass of neural tissue (such as cortex) and
not on any specific part of the mass (Lashley, 1929). There were in principle
two ways in which the neurons in the masses might operate. Either they; formed
discrete, specific networks of selected connections that transmitted pulses
along axons from neuron to neuron across synaptic connections, but with many
distributed parallel or redundant channels for reliable transmission (von
Neumann, 1958; Cowan, 1967). Or they formed cooperative domains by synaptic
interactions over large regions, in which the pattern of activity was specific,
but within certain limits the neurons sustaining it were not. Networks and masses
constructed according to. these two hypotheses shared the same properties of
(1) transmission between neurons solely on the basis of pulses acting at
synapses; (2) immunity of stimulus-response patterns to fairly large
destructive lesions; (3) the occurrence of coherent activity among many neurons
in correlation with a stimulus-response event; and (4) modification of
transmission at specific synapses in the process of learning. Neither lesions
nor recording of coherent activity could answer the question: Was the single
neuron pulse train sufficient as well as necessary to carry the message, or was
the message conveyed in mass action? 6. Bullock (1959) has argued persuasively
that "all-or-none" pulse transmission phylogenetically is relatively
new in comparison to graded wave-like events occurring in primitive and
not-so-primitive nerve nets. Werblin (1971) has shown that transmission occurs
though bipolar neurons 'in the retina of the salamander by graded events. In
the olfactory bulb the granule cells have no axons and do not generate
extracellularly detectable action potentials. These observations imply that it
is not true that the nerve impulse is the "only basis for
transmission" either' between neurons or within neurons. However, they are
not relevant to the question at issue here, because the mode of transmission
(pulse or wave) by one cell does not determine the content, nor does it
preclude reduction of description of the operation to pulse logic. McCulloch (1951) was
explicit on this point: "Now, why have I chosen to quantize in nervous
impulses? Well, let's say the human brain is of a general order of complexity
of something like 10' if we think of it in terms of its ultimate particles. One
might split this at the level of the atoms, or one might split this at the
level of the neurons, and so on. The question is: At what level can one split
the behavior so as to define a set of units in terms of which to work? And,
obviously, the nervous impulse at the level of the neurons is a fairly nice
unit for working. . . . "I
look on . . . a field . . . as an analogy device when we disregard the unitary
composition, or definite structure of our elements, and treat them as wholes,
which means that we are dealing with their parts, if you will, at best,
statistically. We have, with respect to them, thrown away the information that
goes into their construction, and have retained only the overall picture of
their behavior. Now, I do not like to treat something like the cerebral cortex
in this 'field' manner. First, and foremost, I will admit unquestionably that
it has some statistical properties. But what I am looking for is something that
will perform a logical task. I like to look for it in a thing that has a grain,
and I like to take that grain as my unit, because then I can see what degrees
of freedom the system has and to what extent they are bound in any given job of
handling information. I believe that there are many things in the nervous
system that. smear the results .... "They rob the system
of some of its degrees of freedom without conveying information. They are the
kinds of things that would arise if we had to build our electronic devices and
keep them in tubs of salt water.... Now, the reason for making this distinction
as sharp as possible in the face of plenty of evidence that the smearing
occurs, is that we are dealing with something that has a field-Eke property.
My-reason for treating it in the manner in which I have treated it is this. It
compels one so to state his theory that he can account for these changes by the
digital mechanisms; that is, so that he can contrive a digital device to
account for each of these.... It was clearly shown in our first paper, I
believe, that these field-like processes--facilitation and extinction-could be
handled by sticking in hypothetical nets, because once a system is, in this
sense, quantized anywhere, it might as well be quantized everywhere. This keeps
the theory clean" (pp. 132-134). 7. By "wave function" is meant a
continuously and often periodically varying probability of pulse occurrence in
both time and the surface dimensions of the neural mass and not a wave of
polarization along the axon, as the action potential is sometimes referred to. 8. Mere size of mass and number of neurons
are insufficient grounds for invoking the continuum; it is the topology of
interconnection density that is important (Maynard, 1967). Bullock and Horridge
(1965) wrote: ". . . there appears to be a general difference between the
brain waves in invertebrates and vertebrates (Bullock, 1945). From fish to man
. . . the brain exhibits smooth, low-frequency, sinusoidal waves dominated by
rhythms of less than 50 per second and mostly less than 10 per second.... But
in all invertebrates examined, including annelids, arthropods, and molluscs,
activity recorded by surface electrodes is spiky. . . . Comparing the large
cephalothoracic ganglionic mass of Limulus and the brain of a small frog, it
can be concluded that size of ganglion is not the critical difference, and on
similar grounds it seems possible to rule out size of axons. Apart from the spikes,
invertebrate ganglia do show slow waves, and in some cases these appear to
demand something beyond an envelope of individual spikes to account for them .
. ." (p. 318). Certain molluscs and insects such as squid, wasps, and bees
have massive central ganglia. with patterns of anatomical organization similar
to the laminated neuropil (cortex) of higher vertebrates. These animals also
show learning behavior involving pattern recognition similar to that in
vertebrates. Wave activity has been reported in recordings from these
structures (Bullock, 1945; Mislin, 1955; Bullock and Horridge, 1965) but not in
sufficient detail to determine whether neural populations and cartels exist in
these exceptional masses. 9. The point must. be re-emphasized that the
a.d.f. is a mathematical description of an active state which is neither
identical with nor reflected directly in the EEG or single neural pulse trains
but reflected only in statistical averages of them. The rules for obtaining
averages are as yet strictly empirical. It is known that the averages may be
temporal, spatial, or both. Typically the EEG and the evoked potential are
already spatial averages, owing to the fact that they manifest the sum of
extracellular dendritic current from many neurons. They are also time averages,
because of the smoothing action of the resistive-capacitive property of the
dendritic membranes. Whereas the peak of the energy spectrum of the action
potential is near 1000 Hz, that for the AEP or EEG rarely exceeds 80 Hz.
Therefore in contrast to single neuron pulse trains, a short period of
experimental time-averaging suffices to yield a defined activity pattern. Well-defined and
reproducible AEP's from the bulb or cortex require the sum of 30 to 100 trials.
Relatively noise-free PST histograms from single neurons require 1000 to 10,000
trials. Recordings from an optimally placed extracellular EEG electrode in the
bulb reflect the summed dendritic current of about 600,000 granule cells within
a radius of half a millimeter. The same surface area encloses about 1500 mitral
and tufted cells. The spectral and amplitude probability densities of the EEG
can be defined adequately from records 2 to 3 seconds in duration with a sample
rate of 200 per second (400 to 600 samples). The statistical properties of
background pulse trains from single neurons become reasonably clear only after
100 to 300 seconds of recording while sampling for pulses with a sample rate of
1000 per second (100,000 to 300,000 samples). Proportionately fewer
samples are needed when pulse trains are recorded simultaneously by the same
electrode from multiple (3 to 5) neurons, which is a form of spatial averaging
of the pulse trains. These figures from anesthetized animals show that the
amount of averaging required either within or outside the experimental
preparation is prodigious. The reason is that the shared variance among neurons
in the population is an exceedingly small fraction of the total variance of
activity of each neuron within the population. The necessity for computers here
is obvious. The hypothesis underlying
the procedure of experimental time-averaging of pulse trains is that the
sampled neuron will reflect the state of all the neurons of the same population
in its vicinity, if the sample period is long enough, which is in essence the
ergodic hypothesis. But the ergodic hypothesis seems not to hold in any simple
sense or over the designated time range, because the mean pulse rates, Po,
differ from different neurons in the near vicinity. Largely for this reason a
reliable method for estimating the overall mean pulse rate of the population,
Po, has not yet been devised. More fundamental than the
experimental sampling problems is the question, How does the nervous system
read the a.d.f. for the unique sensory event? Time-averaging over more than 100
msec seems unlikely; spatial averaging would seem to be essential. The bulb
reduces the activity of 100,000,000 granule cells to that of 80,000 mitral
cells, and 176 mm' of bulbar surface area to about 1.0 mm', the cross-sectional
area of the LOT. The number of readout lines (80,000) still far exceeds the
number of recording channels available to this experimentalist (1 to 64), so
that his access to the uniquely distributed sensory event without recourse to
repetition and time averaging is not yet possible. It may never be. 10. The possibility has been entertained for
many years that the currents of the EEG (including very low frequency or
"de" potentials) might have direct "ephaptic" effects on
cortical neurons (Köhler and Wallach, 1944; Terzuolo and Bullock, 1956; Adey,
1966, p. 34). This mechanism is specifically excluded from the neural mass
hypothesis here proposed on the following grounds: (1) It is not needed to
account for cooperative domains. (2) Synaptic inputs not only are demonstrably
more effective in most contexts as the basis for neural interactions but are
more likely to be sites for specific changes associated with learning; and, as
Lashley and Hebb stated (see Note 4), such sites must be anatomically specific.
(3) The effects of widespread ephaptic interactions would be to promote
synchronous pulse discharge, but to the extent that ephaptic synchronization
occurred, with each EEG wave activity would be desynchronized on each
subsequent passage through synaptic loops. Although it is conceivable
that ephaptic effects might be responsible for second order cooperative
phenomena, despite psychophysical evidence to the contrary offered by Sperry
and Miner (1955) and by Lashley et al. (1951), it is important to divorce that
question from the problem of describing the dynamics of neural masses, and to
answer it separately. The author wishes to add that each of his own
experimental attempts to demonstrate a significant role for ephaptic
transmission in masses has ended with a strong negative answer. These efforts
have included (1) application of exogenous sinusoidal currents having spatial
and temporal amplitude distributions resembling those of the EEG to animals
that were trained to be exquisitely sensitive to brain electrical stimulation,
whereupon such currents had no behavioral effects (Freeman, 1962); (2)
measurement of cortical impedances to within 0.1% relative magnitude and 0.1°
phase angle (Freeman, 1963) and finding no changes associated with behavior
other than sleep and death; (3) measurement of temporal dispersion among action
potentials in the PON, which was precisely in accord with predictions based on
axon diameter distribution and allowed for no ephaptic synchronization
(Freeman, 1969); and (4) measurement of the effects of applied transcortical
direct currents on the shape of the AEP, which indicate that the current
densities required to induce even modest changes in AEP waveform were one or
two orders of magnitude greater than those physiologically present (Freeman,
unpublished data, 1968). The author has concluded that the mechanism of
ephaptic transmission should not be introduced into a theory of neural masses
until all reasonable synaptic alternatives have been exhausted. 11. LeGros Clark in 1957 wrote: ". . .
The sharply circumscribed nature of the glomerular formations and of their
connexions suggests very forcibly some degree of individualization in their
olfactory functions (in other words, some degree of functional localization) .
. . . In fact, there is some reason for supposing that there may be two modes
of localization in the olfactory bulb, a topical localization in the sense that
certain local areas of the olfactory epithelium are projected to local regions
of the bulb, and another kind of localization whereby fibres derived from
receptors of different physiological significance are predominantly
concentrated on correspondingly different glomeruli. . . . However, there is
another possible explanation of the olfactory nerve plexus in the surface of
the bulb-that it serves precisely the opposite purpose of ensuring complete
randomization of fibres so that each glomerulus receives impulses from every
type of receptor; in other words, that each glomerulus is in itself a
functional unit concerned with the total range of olfactory discrimination. . .
. The possibility has occurred to me that a purely random distribution of
fibres from different specific types of receptors might itself confer some
degree of functional specificity on individual glomeruli if one could suppose
that in a normal distribution the proportion of fibres from each type
terminating in each one of the two thousand glomeruli varies over a
sufficiently wide range. But, assuming that the different types of receptors
are distributed at random in a population of fifty million, statistical
computations show that the existence of several hundred types would need to be
postulated to allow the expectation that the range of variation in the number
of fibres from any one type reaching individual glomeruli would be likely to
have importance from the functional point of view. . . . There is at present no
warrant for supposing that so many different types of receptors do exist; on
the other hand, the possibility is not to be excluded, and it is also possible,
of course, that such receptor types as do exist are not distributed at random
in the olfactory epithelium. . . . The restriction in mammals of the main
dendrite of each mitral cell usually to a single glomerulus suggests rather forcibly
that this is part of a progressive evolutionary development in the specificity
of the glomerular system associated with an enhancement of the capacity for
olfactory discrimination. . . . ". . . The question
arises, then, whether the impulses relayed from the different glomeruli are
delivered in some sort of spatial pattern to different loci in the main
olfactory centres or whether, as seems more likely, the mitral axons which
convey them are distributed in such a way as to give rise to different patterns
of excitation centrally" (pp. 310-314). Since this was written,
Amoore (1971) has collected a large body of data concerning the steric
configurations of odorous molecules, odor similarities, and specific anosmias.
He hypothesizes that particular molecules fit into receptor sites on olfactory
cilia as does a key into a lock. From his present evidence he believes that
there are at least sixty-two specific anosmias; this number approaches the
requirement proposed by LeGros Clark for an olfactory model based on random
connectives between the receptors and neurons in the first synaptic relay.
However, Amoore has also concluded that the number of "primary odors"
in man "may be reduced to twenty-seven through grouping of likely
redundancies" (p. 255). 12. For example, the application of
polarizing direct current to the surface of the prepyriform cortex modifies the
of polarizing direct current to the surface of the prepyriform cortex modifies
the frequency and decay rate of the AEP in the same manner as does external
excitatory or inhibitory bias (Freeman, unpublished results, 1968). It has also
been possible by this means to cause a nonoscillatory AEP (open loop) to become
oscillatory (closed loop), but it is not known whether this induced a traveling
wave condition as well. Some slow or "dc" cortical potentials
(Rowland, 1968) undoubtedly are manifestations of population a.d.f.'s and of
the bias levels they represent. The close association of "dc" shifts
with learning events suggests that a change in a population bias level might
accompany or even be a. necessary condition for such events, and that
"dc" polarization might mimic the effect of a change in bias level,
leading to the formation of an orienting or a conditioned reflex (Rusinov,
1953; Morrell, 1961). 13. The dramatic properties of holograms have
suggested the use of optical analogies for their predictive value. The possible
relevance of the holograph (Gabor, 1947) to brain function has been noted
frequently in the past decade (van Heerden, 1963; Lettvin and Gesteland, 1965;
Longuet-Higgins, 1968; Pribram, 1969; Westlake, 1970; Pollen et al., 1971), as
distinct from interactive networks (Willshaw et al., 1969; Anderson, 1970).
There is an attractive but superficial plausibility about this. In a generic
sense holography is the study of waves, their modes of propagation, their
transformations to form distinctive patterns, and the nonlocalized storage and
regeneration of information in such patterns (Caulfield and Lu, 1970). The
neural populations of the olfactory system, owing to their vast numbers and the
densities of their interconnections, provide the continuous media and the
equivalent of energy density functions required to sustain waves of neural
activity. The neural cartels generate the requisite carrier frequencies and
phase standards. Excitatory populations provide the bias controls needed to
make those frequencies invariant with respect to input amplitudes, and possibly
to adapt them to as-yet-unspecified standard frequency values under central
control. Inhibitory populations determine the stability properties in both time
and space dimensions. The broad divergence of interconnections, established by
both anatomical and physiological measurements, provides the basis for surface
convolution and wave propagation at velocities precisely dependent on axonal
propagation delays. The superposition properties of populations provide the
basis for linear summation and the development of surface interference
patterns. The essential feature of
the application of holography to the sensory process is the conversion of a
sensory distribution (a pattern of light, a manifold of odors, the sound of a
word, etc.) into its Fourier components prior to processing and storage. One
may, in addition, postulate the inverse transform as the basis for image
reconstitution, or further integral transforms into a manifold of motor
patterns on the output side of the brain. It seems undeniable that, if
distributed interactions are in some places the basis for neural information
processing, then, irrespective of the mechanism, the processing can be
described for some purposes in terms of the Fourier transform. So can speech. The difficulty with this
approach is to know whether it has predictive value or merely provides a change
in variables, as spectral analysis does for the EEG. The most attractive
feature of optical holography as an analogy is the mathematical simplicity of
the classical Fourier transform; without this the explanatory mechanisms become
objects of study in themselves. But this simplicity holds only for the case of
far-field (Fraunhofer) diffraction. For near-field (Fresnel) diffraction the
quadrature terms cannot be neglected; and this holds when, as in the olfactory
system, the aperature is probably too small with respect to wavelength to
permit neglect of edge effects, and the output of each element in the object
plane is to some part and not all parts of the image plane. Considering such
neurophysiological complexities as the multiple types of feedback, the
nonlinearities of pulse-to-wave and wave-to-pulse conversion, and the broad
nonlinear coupling that must underlie the phenomenon of bulbar hypersynchrony,
the Fresnel diffraction approach seems more like a Procrustean bed than a
working conceptual tool. An alternative analogy of
great interest is offered by thermodynamics of irreversible processes applied
to "dissipative structures" (Katchalsky, 1971, and personal
communication, 1971). The risks are appreciable of once again confusing neural
massive activity with energy and neural waves with electromagnetic or fluid
waves, but the usefulness of partial differential equations in both domains
seems overriding, particularly as the means for predicting and measuring those
properties of a system at a higher level, which emerge from the strong
interaction of massive numbers of parts at a lower level in a multilevel
hierarchy. REFERENCES Adey, W. R. (1966). Progr. Physiol. Psychol.
1, 1. Adrian, E. D. (1947). "The Physical
Background of Perception." Oxford Univ. Press (Clarendon), London and New
York. Adrian, E. D. (1950). Brit. Med. Bull. 6,
330. Adrian, E. D. (1953). Acta Physiol. Scand.
29, 5. Amoore, J. E. (1971). In "Handbook of
Sensory Physiology IV, Chemical Senses, Part 1, Olfaction" (L. M. Beidler,
ed.), p. 245. Springer-Verlag, Berlin and New York. Anderson, J. A. (1970). Math. Biosci. 8, 137. Armett, C. J., Gray, J. A. B., Hunsperger, R.
W., and Lai, S. (1962). J. Physiol. (London) 164, 395. Barlow, H. B. (1953). J. Physiol. (London)
119, 69. Barlow, H. B. (1969). In "Information
Processing in the Nervous System" (K. N. Liebovic, ed.), Chapter 11.
Springer-Verlag, Berlin and New York. Barlow, R. B., Jr. (1969). J. Gen. Physiol.
54, 383. Bartley, S. H. (1959). In "Handbook of
Physiology, Sect. 1, Neurophysiology," Vol. 1, Chapter 30. Amer. Physiol.
Soc., Washington, D. C. Beurle, R. L. (1956). Phil. Trans. B 240, 55. Blakemore, C. (1970). J. Physiol. (London)
209, 155. Boudreau, J. C. (1964). Nature (London) 201,
155. Boudreau, J. C., and Freeman, W. J. (1963).
Exp. Neurol. 8, 423. Brazier, M. A. B. (1959). In "Handbook
of Physiology, Sect. 1, Neurophysiology," Vol. I, Chapter 1. Amer.
Physiol. Soc., Washington, D. C. Bullock, T. H. (1945). Yale J. Biol. Med. 17,
657. Bullock, T. H. (1959). Science 129, 997. Bullock, T. H., and Horridge, G. A. (1965).
"Structure and Function in the Nervous Systems of Invertebrates,"
Vol. 1. Freeman, San Francisco, California. Caianiello, E. R. (ed.). (1967). "Neural
Network." Springer-Verlag, Berlin and New York. Calvin, W. H., and Stevens, C. F. (1968). J.
Neurophysiol. 31, 524. Campbell, F. W., and Maffei, L. (1970). J.
Physiol. (London) 207, 635. Caulfield, H. G., and, Lu, S. (1970).
"The Application of Holography." Wiley, New York. Chang, H.-T. (1951). J. Neurophysiol. 14, 1. Cowan, J. D. (1967). In "Neural
Networks" (E. R. Caianiello, ed.), pp. 181-188. Springer-Verlag, Berlin
and New York. Cragg, B. G., and Temperley, H. N. V. (1954).
Electroencephalog. Clin. Neurophysiol. 6, 85. Darwin, C. (1872). "Expression of the
Emotions in Man and Animals." John Murray. London. Denny-Brown, D. (1940). "Selected
Writing of Sir Charles Sherrington." Harper (Hoeber), New York. Dravniecks, A. (1964). Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.
116, 429. Eccles, J. C. (1952). "The
Neurophysiological Basis of Mind." Oxford Univ. Press (Clarendon), London
and New York. Eccles, J. C. (1957). "The Physiology of
Nerve Cells," Chapter 11 (Fig. 21), Chapter III (Figs. 39, 45). Johns
Hopkins Press, Baltimore, Maryland. Eccles,. J. C. (1964). "The Physiology
of Synapses," Chapter 1. Academic Press, New York. Eccles, J. C., Ito, M., and SzentAgothai, J.
(1967). "The Cerebellum as Neuronal Machine." Springer-Verlag, Berlin
and New York. Emery, J., and Freeman, W. J. (1969).
Physiol. Behav. 4, 69. Freeman, W. J. (1960). J. Neurophysiol. 23,
111. Freeman, W. J. (1961). In "Neural
Aspects of Temperature Regulation" (J. P. Hannon and E. Viereck, eds.), p.
255. Arctic Aeromedical Lab., Fairbanks, Alaska. Freeman, W. J. (1962). Exp. Neurol. 6, 12. Freeman, W. J. (1963). Int. Rev. Neurobiol.
5, 53. Freeman, W. J. (1964). Exp. Neurol. 10, 525. Freeman, W. J. (1967). Logistics Rev. 3, 5. Freeman, W. J. (1968a). J. Neurophysiol. 31,
1. Freeman, W. J. (1968b). J. Neurophysiol. 31,
337. Freeman, W. J. (1969). Physiologist 12, 229. Freeman, W. J. (1970). In "Approaches to
Neural Modeling" (M. A. B. Brazier and D. Walter, eds.). Brain Information
Service, UCLA, Los Angeles, California. In press. Freeman, W. J. (1972). Ann. Rev. Biophys.
Bioeng. 1, 225. Fulton, J. F. (1949). "Physiology of the
Nervous System." Oxford Univ. Press, London and New York. Gabor, D. (1947). Proc. Roy. Soc. Ser. A 197,
454. Gerstein, G. L., and Kiang, N. Y.-S. (1960).
Biophys. J. 1, 15. Grafstein, B. (1956). J. Neurophysiol. 19,
154. Granit, R. (1967). "Charles Scott
Sherrington-An Appraisal," p. 134. Doubleday, Garden City, New York. Granit, R., Kernell, D., and Shortess, G. K.
(1963). J. Physiol. (London) 168, 911. Hartline, H. K. (1938). Amer. J. Physiol.
121, 400. Hartline, H. K., and Ratliff, F. (1959). J.
Gen. Physiol. 41, 1049. Hebb, D. 0. (1949). "The Organization of
Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory." Wiley, New York. Heimer, L. (1968). J. Anat. 103, 413. Heimer, L. (1969). Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 167,
129. Horowitz, J. M., and Freeman, W. J. (1966).
Bull. Math. Biophys. 28, 519. Horowitz, J. M. (1972). Electroencephalog.
Clin. Neurophysiol. 32, 227. Householder, A. S., and Landahl, H. D.
(1945). "Mathematical Biophysics of the Central Nervous System."
Principia, Bloomington, Indiana. Hubel, D. H., and Wiesel, T. N. (1963) J.
Physiol. (London) 165, 559. John, E. R. (1967). "Mechanisms of
Memory." Academic Press, New York. Jung, R. (1961). In "Sensory
Communication" (W. A. Rosenblith, ed.), Chapter 32. MIT Press, Cambridge,
Massachusetts. Kandel, E. R., and Spencer, W. A. (1961).
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 94, 570. Katchalsky, A. (1971). Neurosci. Res. Progr.
Bull. 9, 397. Katz, B. (1939). "Electrical Excitation
of Nerve." Oxford Univ. Press, London and New York. Kiang, N. Y.-S. (1968). "Discharge
Patterns of Single Fibers in the Cat's AuditorN Nerve." MIT Press,
Cambridge, Massachusetts. Kirschfeld, K., and Reichardt, W. (1964).
Kybernetik 2, 43. Knight, B. W., Toyada, J., and Dodge, F. A.,
Jr. (1970). J. Gen. Physiol. 56, 421. Köhler, W. (1940). "Dynamics in
Psychology." Grove Press, New York. Köhler, W., and, Wallach, H. (1944). Proc.
Amer. Phil. Ass. 88, 269. Lashley, K. S. (1929). "Brain Mechanisms
and Intelligence, A Quantitative Study of Injuries to the Brain." Univ. of
Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois. Lashley, K. S. (1942). Biol. Symp. 7, 301. Lashley, K. S., Chow, K. L., and Semmes, J.
(1951). Psychol. Rev. 58, 123. Leão, A. A. P. (1944). J. Neurophysiol. 7,
359. LeGros Clark, W., (1951). J. Neurol.
Psychiat. 14, 1. LeGros Clark, W. (1956). Yale J. Biol. Med.
29, 83. LeGros Clark, W. (1957). Proc. Roy. Soc. Ser.
B. 146, 299. Lettvin, J. Y., and Gesteland, R. C. (1965).
Cold Spring Harbor Symp. Quant. Biol. 30, 217. Lettvin, J. Y., Maturana, H. R., McCulloch,
W. S., and Pitts, W. H. (1959). Proc. IRE 47, 1940. Liebovic, K. N. (ed.). (1969).
"Information Processing in the Nervous System." Springer-Verlag,
Berlin and New York. Lilly, J. C., and Cherry, R. B. (1954). J. Neurophysiol.
17, 521. Longuet-Higgins, H. C. (1968). Proc. Roy.
Soc. Ser. B 171, 327. Lorente de Nó, R. (1933). Arch. Neurol.
Psychiat. 30, 245. Lorente de Nó, R. (1938). J. Neurophysiol. 1,
207. Marr, D. (1969). J. Physiol. (London) 202,
437. Marshall, W. H., and Talbot, S. A. (1942).
Biol. Symp. 7, 117. Maynard, D. M. (1967). In "Invertebrate
Nervous System" (C. A. G. Wiersma, ed.), Chapter 18. Univ. of Chicago
Press, Chicago, Illinois. McCulloch, W. S. (1951). In "Cerebral
Mechanisms in Behavior" (L. A. Jeffress, ed.), p. 42. Wiley, New York. McCulloch, W. S., and Pitts, W. H. (1943).
Bull. Math. Biophys. 5, 115. Mislin, H. (1955). Acta Med. Scand. Suppl.
307, 58. Morrell, F. (1961). Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 92,
860. Mountcastle, V. B. (1957). J. Neurophysiol.
20, 408. Nicoll, C. A. (1971). Science 171, 824. O'Leary, J. (1937). J. Comp. Neurol. 67, 1. Palay, S. L. (1956). J. Biophys. Biochem.
Cytol. 2(Suppl.), 193. Parzen, E. (1960). "Modern Probability
Theory and Its Applications," p. 60. Wiley, New York. Perkel, D. H., and Bullock, T. H. (1968).
Neural Coding. Neurosci. Res. Progr. 6, No. 3. Peters, A., Palay, S. L., and Webster, H. de
F. (1970). "The Fine Structure of the Nervous System," Chapter 1.
Harper (Hoeber), New York. Phillips, C. G., Powell, T. P. S., and
Shepherd, G. M. (1963). J. Physiol. (London) 168, 65. Poggio, G. F., and Viernstein, L. J. (1964).
J. Neurophysiol. 27, 517. Pollen, D. A., Lee, J. R., and Taylor, J. H.
(1971). Science 173, 74. Pribram, K. H. (1969). Sci. Amer. 220, 73. Pribram, K. H. (1971). "Languages of the
Brain." Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey. Prochaska, G. (1784). Adnotationum
Academicarum. Wolfgangum Gerle, Prague. See also: The Principles of Physiology,
by John Augustus Unzer; and a Dissertation on the Functions of the Nervous
System by George Prochaska (Translated by T. Laycock in 1851). Sydenham Soc.,
London. Prochaska, G. (1812). "Disquisitio
Anatomica-Physiologica Organismi Corporis Humani." de Hakul, Vienna. Rall, W. (1955). J. Cell. Comp. Physiol. 45,
373. Rall, W., and Shepherd, G. M. (1968). J.
Neurophysiol. 31, 884. Rall, W., Shepherd, G. M., Reese, T., and
Brightman, M. W. (1966). Exp. Neurol. 14,44. Ramón y Cajal, S. (1909-1911).
"Histologie du Systeme Nerveux de I'Homme et des Vertebres," Vols. I
and II. Maloine, Paris. Ramón y Cajal, S. (1928). "Degeneration
and Regeneration of the Nervous System" (translated by R. M. May). Oxford
Univ. Press, London and New York. Ramón y Cajal, S. (1955). "Studies on the
Cerebral Cortex (Limbic Structures)" (translated by L. M. Kraft). Yearbook
Publ., Chicago, Illinois. Ranson, S. W., and Hinsey, J. C. (1930).
Amer. J. Physiol. 94, 471. Rodieck, R. W., and Stone, J. (1965). J.
Neurophysiol. 28, 833. Rodieck, R. W., Kiang, N. Y.-S., and
Gerstein, G. L. (1962). Biophys. J. 2, 351. Rosenblatt, F. (1962). "Principles of
Neurodynamics." Spartan Books, Washington, D. C. Rowland, V. (1968). Progr. Physiol. Psychol.
2, 2. Rusinov, V. S. (1953). Commun. 19th Int.
Physiol. Congr. Montreal. Schmitt, F. 0. (ed.) (1970). "The
Neurosciences: Second Study Program." Rockefeller Univ. Press, New York. Shepherd, G. M. (1963). J. Physiol. (London)
168, 101. Sherrington, C. S. (1906). "The
Integrative Action of the Nervous System." Yale Univ. Press, New Haven,
Connecticut. Sherrington, C. (1925). Proc. Roy. Soc. Ser.
B 97, 519. Sherrington, C. (1929). Proc. Roy. Soc. Ser.
B 105, 332. Sholl, D. A. (1956). "The Organization
of the Cerebral Cortex." Methuen, London. Sperry, R. W. (1951). In "Handbook of
Experimental Psychology" (S. S. Stevens, ed.), Chapter 7, p. 236. Wiley,
New York. Sperry, R. W., and Miner, N. (1955). J. Comp.
Physiol. Psychol. 48, 463. Ten Hoopen, M., and Verveen, A. A. (1963).
Progr. Brain Res. 2, 8. Terzuolo, C. A., and Bullock, T. H. (1956).
Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U. S. 42, 687. Valverde, F. (1965). "Studies on the
Piriform Lobe." Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Van Heerden, P. J. (1963). Appl. Opt. 2, 387. von Haller, A. (1776). "Elementa
physiologiae corporis humani," Vol. 4, "Cerebrum, Nervi,
Musculi." Vincentium Ursinum, Neapoli. von Neumann, J. (1958). "The Computer
and the Brain." Yale Univ. Press, New Haven, Connecticut. Voronkov, G. S., and Gusel'nikova, K. G.
(1968). Neurosci. Traml. 4, 425. Wall, P. D. (1962). J. Physiol. (London) 164,
508. Werblin, F. S. (1971). J. Neurophysiol. 34,
228. Westlake, P. R. (1970). Kybernetic 7, 129. White, L. E., Jr. (1965). Anat. Rec. 152,
465. Wiersma, C. A. G. (1967). "Invertebrate
Nervous Systems." Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois. Willshaw, D. J., Buneman, 0. P., and
Longuet-Higgins, H. C. (1969). Nature (London) 222, 960. Wilson, H. R., and Cowan, J. D. (1971).
Abstr. 1st Ann. Meeting, Soc. Neurosci. P-72. Wilson, H. R., and Cowan, J. D. (1972). Biophys.
J. 12, 1.
Walter J. Freeman
Journal Article e-Reprint
This work was supported by National Institute
of Mental Health grant MH 6686. The editorial assistance of Charmane Thomson,
the programming of Brian Burke, and the secretarial assistance of Janice Kidd
are gratefully acknowledged.
cc: Creative Commons Copyright: Attribution-Share alike Licence
Original PDF File Source: http://sulcus.berkeley.edu/FreemanWWW/manuscripts/IA4/72.html
A), each block specifying the probability that a pulse
occurs given a certain time delay from a certain wave amplitude.
to +3
is designated A.., The corresponding ranges for
inhibition (from -1
, to -3
) are called "negative" and Amin.
T), are shown in Fig. 3 (bottom, small open
triangles). These curves are oscillatory, with the same frequency as the
on-going frequency of the EEG. They show the optimum time lag for the three
conversions represented. This time lag is denoted Tma.
for the excitatory neurons, types A and M, and Tmi. for inhibitory
neurons, type B. For example, in the graph at the left it is seen that the
mitral cell is most likely to fire 4 msec prior to the maximum of the granule
cell wave (Tm.. = -4 msec). This is because the mitral cells excite
the granule cells, and the rise in their firing probability is followed by a
wave of granule cell activity.
T...) and P(A Tmi.), are shown in Fig. 3 (top, small open triangles).
These are the experimental input-output curves of pulse-to-wave and
wave-to-pulse conversions. The slopes or derivatives of these curves give the
instantaneous output to input ratios or the conversion factors.
p = 100 microns. A focus of excitation on
the PON of the same form with
a = 200 microns will arrive at the
glomeruli with or, 11 •
a2)1/2 or 665 microns, owing to a dilative divergence. For
each successive synaptic action among periglomerular neurons the Gaussian input
distribution must be convolved with the distribution of the processes of the
periglomerular cells. The convolution can be described quantitatively by the
addition of the variance. After one = (
a2 +
a)1/2, or 670 microns. After twenty interactions the
expected size of the focus sa20 = 800 microns) would be augmented by
less than the size of one glomerulus (about 150 microns). At a cycle duration
of about 5 msec for each transaction, this is about as long as a single-shock
response lasts. The implication is that small neurons in long serial chains
cannot contribute significantly to divergence.
,). The epicenter of the AEP is shown by A, and the
centers of the ellipses by E and I. X. is half-amplitude radius for the AEP.
The values for
are
those for bivariate normal distributions of the a.d.f.'s. Symmetrical radial
spread in anesthetized animals does not occur after the first cycle of
oscillation of the AEP.
i Beginning with the initial region of
excitation, for which all is determined by the variance of the input a.d.f.,
the predicted variance of the next stage,
j2, is equal to the sum of
a2 and
i2, where a. = 382 microns is the mean standard
deviation of the anatomical connection density (as a function of distance) of
the mitral-tufted basal dendrites.
T), determined the "vector" of the neuron
signal. Mitral cell. P, = 12.3 pps.
= 80,000.
T), characteristically conforms to a sinusoidal
oscillation, for which the frequency is the peak frequency of the EEG. -The
fitted curve, P (Amax
T), varies from one neuron to the next in a manner
that, while not random, is not at present satisfactorily predictable. The range
for phase is at least 60º from the mean for the neuron type, and the range for
modulation amplitude is from zero to a magnitude in excess of Po,
clipping then being obvious.
T) leads the EEG by less than 90º, and when it is
dominant in the feedback limb, P. (A....
T) leads the EEG by more than 90º.
This work is licensed under Creative Commons License: Attribution-Share alike.